Results for '*Perceptual Motor Learning'

977 found
Order:
  1.  41
    Effect of addition of irrelevant verbal cues on perceptual-motor learning.Harry W. Braun & A. W. Bendig - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 54 (2):105.
  2.  47
    Role of kinesthetic and spatial-visual abilities in perceptual-motor learning.Edwin A. Fleishman & Simon Rich - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (1):6.
  3.  34
    Supplementary report: Effect of addition of irrelevant verbal cues on perceptual-motor learning.Harry W. Braun & A. W. Bendig - 1958 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 55 (3):301.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  17
    Applications of power functions to perceptual-motor learning.Joseph C. Stevens - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (6):614.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  5.  40
    Attention and time constraints in perceptual-motor learning and performance: Instruction, analogy, and skill level.Johan M. Koedijker, Jamie M. Poolton, Jonathan P. Maxwell, Raôul R. D. Oudejans, Peter J. Beek & Rich S. W. Masters - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (2):245-256.
    We sought to gain more insight into the effects of attention focus and time constraints on skill learning and performance in novices and experts by means of two complementary experiments using a table tennis paradigm. Experiment 1 showed that skill-focus conditions and slowed ball frequency disrupted the accuracy of experts, but dual-task conditions and speeded ball frequency did not. For novices, only speeded ball frequency disrupted accuracy. In Experiment 2, we extended these findings by instructing novices either explicitly or (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  6.  63
    Perceptual-motor constraints on sound-to-meaning correspondence in language.Laura L. Namy & Lynne C. Nygaard - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (5):528-529.
    The proposal that language has evolved to conform to general cognitive and learning constraints inherent in the human brain calls for specification of these mechanisms. We propose that just as cognition appears to be grounded in cross-modal perceptual-motor capabilities, so too must language. Evidence for perceptual-motor grounding comes from non-arbitrary sound-to-meaning correspondences and their role in word learning.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  7.  29
    Perceptual-Cognitive Changes During Motor Learning: The Influence of Mental and Physical Practice on Mental Representation, Gaze Behavior, and Performance of a Complex Action.Cornelia Frank, William M. Land & Thomas Schack - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  8.  31
    Environmental Design Shapes Perceptual-motor Exploration, Learning, and Transfer in Climbing.Ludovic Seifert, Jérémie Boulanger, Dominic Orth & Keith Davids - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  9.  41
    The progression-regression hypotheses in perceptual-motor skill learning.Alfred H. Fuchs - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 63 (2):177.
  10.  11
    The Differential Effects of Auditory and Visual Stimuli on Learning, Retention and Reactivation of a Perceptual-Motor Temporal Sequence in Children With Developmental Coordination Disorder.Mélody Blais, Mélanie Jucla, Stéphanie Maziero, Jean-Michel Albaret, Yves Chaix & Jessica Tallet - 2021 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    This study investigates the procedural learning, retention, and reactivation of temporal sensorimotor sequences in children with and without developmental coordination disorder. Twenty typically-developing children and 12 children with DCD took part in this study. The children were required to tap on a keyboard, synchronizing with auditory or visual stimuli presented as an isochronous temporal sequence, and practice non-isochronous temporal sequences to memorize them. Immediate and delayed retention of the audio-motor and visuo-motor non-isochronous sequences were tested by removing (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  11.  71
    Explicit pre-training instruction does not improve implicit perceptual-motor sequence learning.Daniel J. Sanchez & Paul J. Reber - 2013 - Cognition 126 (3):341-351.
  12.  18
    Beyond the mean reaction time: Trial-by-trial reaction time reveals the distraction effect on perceptual-motor sequence learning.Yue Du & Jane E. Clark - 2020 - Cognition 202 (C):104287.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  40
    Transfer of training in motor learning as a function of distribution of practice.Victor E. Montgomery - 1953 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 46 (6):440.
  14.  18
    Distribution of effort in a perceptual-motor task.E. B. Skaggs - 1935 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 18 (6):797.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  40
    Acquisition and long-term retention of a simple serial perceptual-motor skill.Eva Neumann & R. B. Ammons - 1957 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 53 (3):159.
  16.  71
    Conscious and effortful or effortless and automatic: A practice/performance paradox in motor learning.Flavio T. P. Oliveira & David Goodman - 2004 - Perceptual and Motor Skills 99 (1):315-324.
  17.  59
    Perceptual learning: Differentiation or enrichment?James J. Gibson & Eleanor J. Gibson - 1955 - Psychological Review 62 (1):32-41.
  18.  92
    The Representation of Motor (Inter)action, States of Action, and Learning: Three Perspectives on Motor Learning by Way of Imagery and Execution.Cornelia Frank & Thomas Schack - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8:224812.
    Learning in intelligent systems is a result of direct or indirect interaction with the environment. Humans can learn by way of different states of (inter-)action such as the execution or the imagery of an action, but their unique potential to induce brain-related as well as mind-related changes in the motor action system is still being debated. The systematic repetition of different states of action (e.g., execution and imagery in terms of physical and mental practice) and their contribution to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  19.  35
    Recent insights into perceptual and motor skill learning.Lior Shmuelof & John W. Krakauer - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  20.  53
    Cognitive Representation of a Complex Motor Action Executed by Different Motor Systems.Heiko Lex, Christoph Schütz, Andreas Knoblauch & Thomas Schack - 2015 - Minds and Machines 25 (1):1-15.
    The present study evaluates the cognitive representation of a kicking movement performed by a human and a humanoid robot, and how they are represented in experts and novices of soccer and robotics, respectively. To learn about the expertise-dependent development of memory structures, we compared the representation structures of soccer experts and robot experts concerning a human and humanoid robot kicking movement. We found different cognitive representation structures for both expertise groups under two different motor performance conditions . In general, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  21.  15
    The effect of haptic cues on motor and perceptual based implicit sequence learning.Dongwon Kim, Brandon J. Johnson, R. Brent Gillespie & Rachael D. Seidler - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  22. Non-consciously controlled decision making for fast motor reactions in sports--a priming approach for motor responses to non-consciously perceived movement features.Armin Kibele - 2006 - Psychology of Sport and Exercise 7 (6):591-610.
  23.  49
    No difference between conscious and nonconscious visuomotor control: Evidence from perceptual learning in the masked prime task☆.Friederike Schlaghecken, Elisabeth Blagrove & Elizabeth A. Maylor - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (1):84-93.
    Negative compatibility effects in the masked-prime paradigm are usually obtained when primes are masked effectively. With ineffective masks—and primes above the perceptual threshold—positive compatibility effects occur. We investigated whether this pattern reflects a causal relationship between conscious awareness and low-level motor control, or whether it reflects the fact that both are affected in the same way by changes in physical stimulus attributes. In a 5-session perceptual learning task, participants learned to consciously identify masked primes. However, they showed unaltered (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  24. From Perceptual Categories to Concepts: What Develops?Vladimir M. Sloutsky - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (7):1244-1286.
    People are remarkably smart: They use language, possess complex motor skills, make nontrivial inferences, develop and use scientific theories, make laws, and adapt to complex dynamic environments. Much of this knowledge requires concepts and this study focuses on how people acquire concepts. It is argued that conceptual development progresses from simple perceptual grouping to highly abstract scientific concepts. This proposal of conceptual development has four parts. First, it is argued that categories in the world have different structure. Second, there (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  25. Evaluating Weaknesses of “Perceptual-Cognitive Training” and “Brain Training” Methods in Sport: An Ecological Dynamics Critique.Ian Renshaw, Keith Davids, Duarte Araújo, Ana Lucas, William M. Roberts, Daniel J. Newcombe & Benjamin Franks - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    The recent upsurge in “brain-training and perceptual-cognitive-training", proposing to improve isolated processes such as brain function, visual perception and decision-making, has created significant interest in elite sports practitioners, seeking to create an ‘edge’ for athletes. The claims of these related 'performance-enhancing industries' can be considered together as part of a process training approach proposing enhanced cognitive and perceptual skills and brain capacity, to support performance in everyday life activities, including sport. For example, the 'process-training industry' promotes the idea that playing (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  26.  22
    Neural Suppression Elicited During Motor Imagery Following the Observation of Biological Motion From Point-Light Walker Stimuli.Alice Grazia, Michael Wimmer, Gernot R. Müller-Putz & Selina C. Wriessnegger - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 15.
    Introduction: Advantageous effects of biological motion detection, a low-perceptual mechanism that allows the rapid recognition and understanding of spatiotemporal characteristics of movement via salient kinematics information, can be amplified when combined with motor imagery, i.e., the mental simulation of motor acts. According to Jeannerod’s neurostimulation theory, asynchronous firing and reduction of mu and beta rhythm oscillations, referred to as suppression over the sensorimotor area, are sensitive to both MI and action observation of BM. Yet, not many studies investigated (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  16
    Cognitive Development as a Piece of the Language Learning Puzzle.Eleonore H. M. Smalle & Riikka Möttönen - 2023 - Cognitive Science 47 (5):e13296.
    Why do children learn language more easily than adults do? This puzzle has fascinated cognitive and language scientists for decades. In the present letter, we approach the language learning puzzle from a cognitive perspective that is inspired by evidence from the perceptual and motor learning literature. Neuroscientific studies show that two memory systems in the brain are involved in human learning: an early implicit procedural memory system and a late-developing cognitive or declarative memory system. We argue (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  28.  28
    Implicit visual learning and the expression of learning.Hilde Haider, Katharina Eberhardt, Alexander Kunde & Michael Rose - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (1):82-98.
    Although the existence of implicit motor learning is now widely accepted, the findings concerning perceptual implicit learning are ambiguous. Some researchers have observed perceptual learning whereas other authors have not. The review of the literature provides different reasons to explain this ambiguous picture, such as differences in the underlying learning processes, selective attention, or differences in the difficulty to express this knowledge. In three experiments, we investigated implicit visual learning within the original serial reaction (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  29.  59
    Learning and exploration: Lessons from infants.Karen E. Adolph, Ludovic M. Marin & Frederic F. Fraisse - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (2):213-214.
    Based on studies with infants, we expand on Stoffregen & Bardy's explanation of perceptual motor errors, given the global array. Information pick-up from the global array is not sufficient without adequate exploratory movements and learning to support perceptually guided activity.
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  16
    Implicit Learning of True and False Belief Sequences.Qianying Ma, Elien Heleven, Giulia Funghi, Min Pu, Kris Baetens, Natacha Deroost & Frank Van Overwalle - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    To investigate whether people can implicitly learn regularities in a social context, we developed a new implicit sequence learning task combining elements from classic false belief and serial reaction time tasks. Participants learned that protagonists were offered flowers at four locations. The protagonists' beliefs concerning the flowers were true or false, depending on their orientation, respectively, toward the scene or away from it. Unbeknown to the participants, there was a fixed belief-related sequence involving three dimensions. Participants had to indicate (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  97
    Anti-Intellectualism for the Learning and Employment of Skill.Daniel C. Burnston - 2020 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 12 (3):507-526.
    I draw on empirical results from perceptual and motor learning to argue for an anti-intellectualist position on skill. Anti-intellectualists claim that skill or know-how is non-propositional. Recent proponents of the view have stressed the flexible but fine-grained nature of skilled control as supporting their position. However, they have left the nature of the mental representations underlying such control undertheorized. This leaves open several possible strategies for the intellectualist, particularly with regard to skill learning. Propositional knowledge may structure (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  32.  70
    Dimension‐Based Statistical Learning Affects Both Speech Perception and Production.Matthew Lehet & Lori L. Holt - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (S4):885-912.
    Multiple acoustic dimensions signal speech categories. However, dimensions vary in their informativeness; some are more diagnostic of category membership than others. Speech categorization reflects these dimensional regularities such that diagnostic dimensions carry more “perceptual weight” and more effectively signal category membership to native listeners. Yet perceptual weights are malleable. When short-term experience deviates from long-term language norms, such as in a foreign accent, the perceptual weight of acoustic dimensions in signaling speech category membership rapidly adjusts. The present study investigated whether (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  33.  34
    Knowledge of one’s kinematics improves perceptual discrimination.Elena Daprati, Selina Wriessnegger & Francesco Lacquaniti - 2007 - Consciousness and Cognition 16 (1):178-188.
    We tested the hypothesis that our ability to detect fine kinematics variations is tuned to reveal more subtle differences when the motion pattern belongs to the observer compared to another individual. To this purpose, we analyzed the responses of 15 subjects in a same-different task on pairs of movements, which could belong to one or two different subjects. Self vs. Other comparisons were obtained by presenting both the observer’s and another participant’s kinematics. Subjects responded faster and more accurately when they (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  34.  14
    Scene Matching Method for Children’s Psychological Distress Based on Deep Learning Algorithm.Junli Su - 2021 - Complexity 2021:1-11.
    In the process of children’s psychological development, various levels of psychological distress often occur, such as attention problems, emotional problems, adaptation problems, language problems, and motor coordination problems; these problems have seriously affected children’s healthy growth. Scene matching in the treatment of psychological distress can prompt children to change from a third-person perspective to a first-person perspective and shorten the distance between scene contents and child’s perceptual experience. As a part of machine learning, deep learning can perform (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35. Christian Mannes.Learning Sensory-Motor Coordination Experimentation - 1990 - In G. Dorffner (ed.), Konnektionismus in Artificial Intelligence Und Kognitionsforschung. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. pp. 95.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  7
    Pedagogia per educare.Mario Ferracuti - 1985 - Casale Monferrato, AL: Edizioni progetto scuola.
  37. Response priming with and without awareness.Jens Schwarzbach & Dirk Vorberg - 2006 - In Haluk O. Gmen & Bruno G. Breitmeyer (eds.), The First Half Second: The Microgenesis and Temporal Dynamics of Unconscious and Conscious Visual Processes. MIT Press. pp. 297-314.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  63
    The cerebral representation of space: insights from functional imaging data.Eleanor A. Maguire - 1997 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1 (2):62-68.
    Functional imaging techniques, such as positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging, present a unique opportunity to examine, in humans, the cerebral representation of space in vivo. Space is ubiquitous and not a unitary phenomenon, and the brain uses visual, vestibular and proprioceptive inputs to produce multiple representations of space subserving spatial cognition, ranging from gaze control to remembering multiple complex large-scale environments. Functional imaging studies have shown the importance of the parietal cortex in perceptual, motor, attention and (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  49
    Language is shaped for social interactions, as well as by the brain.Mikkel Wallentin & Chris D. Frith - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (5):536-537.
    Language learning is not primarily driven by a motivation to describe invariant features of the world, but rather by a strong force to be a part of the social group, which by definition is not invariant. It is not sufficient for language to be fit for the speaker's perceptual motor system. It must also be fit for social interactions.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  40.  27
    The Emergence of Discrete Perceptual-Motor Units in a Production Model That Assumes Holistic Phonological Representations.Maya Davis & Melissa A. Redford - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10:468824.
    Intelligible speakers achieve specific vocal tract constrictions in rapid sequence. These constrictions are associated in theory with speech motor goals. Adult-focused models of speech production assume that discrete phonological representations, sequenced into word-length plans for output, define these goals. This assumption introduces a serial order problem for speech. It is also at odds with children's speech. In particular, child phonology and timing control suggest holistic speech plans, and so the hypothesis of whole word production. This hypothesis solves the serial (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  41.  44
    Robot-Assisted Training of the Kinesthetic Sense: Enhancing Proprioception after Stroke.Dalia De Santis, Jacopo Zenzeri, Maura Casadio, Lorenzo Masia, Assunta Riva, Pietro Morasso & Valentina Squeri - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8:119835.
    Proprioception has a crucial role in promoting or hindering motor learning. In particular, an intact position sense strongly correlates with the chances of recovery after stroke. A great majority of neurological patients present both motor dysfunctions and impairments in kinesthesia, but traditional robot and virtual reality training techniques focus either in recovering motor functions or in assessing proprioceptive deficits. An open challenge is to implement effective and reliable tests and training protocols for proprioception that go beyond (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  42.  33
    An Introduction to Predictive Processing Models of Perception and Decision‐Making.Mark Sprevak & Ryan Smith - forthcoming - Topics in Cognitive Science.
    The predictive processing framework includes a broad set of ideas, which might be articulated and developed in a variety of ways, concerning how the brain may leverage predictive models when implementing perception, cognition, decision-making, and motor control. This article provides an up-to-date introduction to the two most influential theories within this framework: predictive coding and active inference. The first half of the paper (Sections 2–5) reviews the evolution of predictive coding, from early ideas about efficient coding in the visual (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43. Motor learning and memory for visually guided reaching.R. Shadmehr & S. P. Wise - 2004 - In Michael S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences III. MIT Press. pp. 353--375.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  28
    Motor learning models.Daniel M. Wolpert & Zoubin Ghahramani - 2003 - In L. Nadel (ed.), Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Nature Publishing Group.
  45.  14
    Motor Learning in Response to Different Experimental Pain Models Among Healthy Individuals: A Systematic Review.Mohammad Izadi, Sae Franklin, Marianna Bellafiore & David W. Franklin - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    Learning new movement patterns is a normal part of daily life, but of critical importance in both sport and rehabilitation. A major question is how different sensory signals are integrated together to give rise to motor adaptation and learning. More specifically, there is growing evidence that pain can give rise to alterations in the learning process. Despite a number of studies investigating the role of pain on the learning process, there is still no systematic review (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  26
    A Perceptual Motor Intervention Improves Play Behavior in Children with Moderate to Severe Cerebral Palsy.Brigette O. Ryalls, Regina Harbourne, Lisa Kelly-Vance, Jordan Wickstrom, Nick Stergiou & Anastasia Kyvelidou - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  47.  17
    Cognitive and Motor Learning in Internally-Guided Motor Skills.Krishn Bera, Anuj Shukla & Raju S. Bapi - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12:604323.
    Several canonical experimental paradigms (e.g., serial reaction time task, discrete sequence production task,m×ntask) have been proposed to study the typical behavioral phenomenon and the nature of learning in sequential keypress tasks. A characteristic feature of most paradigms is that they are representative ofexternally-specifiedsequencing—motor tasks where the environment or task paradigm extrinsically provides the sequence of stimuli, i.e., the responses are stimulus-driven. Previous studies utilizing such canonical paradigms have largely overlooked the learning behaviors in a more realistic class (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  20
    Postrest motor learning performance as a function of degree of learning.John C. Jahnke - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (6):605.
  49.  21
    Modulating Motor Learning through Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation: An Integrative View.Claudia Ammann, Danny Spampinato & Javier Márquez-Ruiz - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  50.  32
    Perceptual-motor performance under rotation of the central field.Daniel W. Smothergill, Richard Martin & Herbert L. Pick - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 87 (1):64.
1 — 50 / 977