Results for 'Ab Markman'

958 found
Order:
  1. Not all differences are the same-similarity as the alignment of conceptual frames.Ab Markman & D. Gentner - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (6):518-518.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2. Reflective and Evaluative Modes of Mental Simulation.Keith D. Markman & Matthew N. McMullen - 2005 - In David R. Mandel, Denis J. Hilton & Patrizia Catellani (eds.), The psychology of counterfactual thinking. New York: Routledge. pp. 77--93.
    A number of researchers have focused on the distinction between upward counterfactuals that simulate a better reality and downward counterfactuals that simulate a worse reality. In this chapter the authors will discuss the important aspects of a model (Markman and McMullen 2003) that attempts to explain how the very same counterfactual can engender dramatically different affective reactions. According to the model, the consequences of simulation direction are moderated by what we have termed simulation mode--relatively stronger tendencies to engage in (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  3. Corporate Citizenship: social responsibility, responsiveness, and performance. In. CARROLL, AB; BUCHHOLTZ, AK.Ab Carroll - forthcoming - Business and Society.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  29
    Constraints Children Place on Word Meanings.Ellen M. Markman - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (1):57-77.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   90 citations  
  5.  22
    Constraints on Analogical Inference.Arthur B. Markman - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (4):373-418.
    The ability to reason by analogy is particularly important because it permits the extension of knowledge of a target domain by virtue of its similarity to a base domain via a process of analogical inference. The general procedure for analogical inference involves copying structure from the base to the target in which missing information is generated, and substitutions are made for items for which analogical correspondences have already been found. A pure copying with substitution and generation process is too profligate (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  6. In defense of representation.Arthur B. Markman & Eric Dietrich - 2000 - Cognitive Psychology 40 (2):138--171.
    The computational paradigm, which has dominated psychology and artificial intelligence since the cognitive revolution, has been a source of intense debate. Recently, several cognitive scientists have argued against this paradigm, not by objecting to computation, but rather by objecting to the notion of representation. Our analysis of these objections reveals that it is not the notion of representation per se that is causing the problem, but rather specific properties of representations as they are used in various psychological theories. Our analysis (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  7.  35
    One alignment mechanism or many?Arthur B. Markman, Kyungil Kim, Levi B. Larkey, Lisa Narvaez & C. Hunt Stilwell - 2004 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (2):204-205.
    Pickering & Garrod (P&G) suggest that communicators synchronize their processing at a number of linguistic levels. Whereas their explanation suggests that representations are being compared across individuals, there must be some representation of all conversation participants in each participant's head. At the level of the situation model, it is important to maintain separate representations for each participant. At other levels, it seems less crucial to have a separate representation for each participant. This analysis suggests that different mechanisms may synchronize representations (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8. Knowledge representation.Arthur B. Markman - 2002 - In J. Wixted & H. Pashler (eds.), Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology. Wiley.
  9.  27
    Why superordinate category terms can be mass nouns.Ellen M. Markman - 1985 - Cognition 19 (1):31-53.
  10.  41
    Information learned from generic language becomes central to children’s biological concepts: Evidence from their open-ended explanations.Andrei Cimpian & Ellen M. Markman - 2009 - Cognition 113 (1):14-25.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  11. Something old, Something new: Extending the classical view of representation.Arthur B. Markman & Eric Dietrich - 2000 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (12):470-475.
    Representation is a central part of models in cognitive science, but recently this idea has come under attack. Researchers advocating perceptual symbol systems, situated action, embodied cognition, and dynamical systems have argued against central assumptions of the classical representational approach to mind. We review the core assumptions of the dominant view of representation and the four suggested alternatives. We argue that representation should remain a core part of cognitive science, but that the insights from these alternative approaches must be incorporated (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12. Cognitive Dynamics: Conceptual change in humans and machines.Eric Dietrich Art Markman (ed.) - 2000 - Lawrence Erlbaum.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  11
    Advertising Genetic Testing.Maurie Markman - 2002 - Hastings Center Report 32 (6):5.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14. Decision making.Arthur B. Markman & Douglas L. Medin - 2002 - In J. Wixted & H. Pashler (eds.), Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology. Wiley.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  15. Whither structured representation?Arthur B. Markman & Eric Dietrich - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4):626-627.
    The perceptual symbol system view assumes that perceptual representations have a role-argument structure. A role-argument structure is often incorporated into amodal symbol systems in order to explain conceptual functions like abstraction and rule use. The power of perceptual symbol systems to support conceptual functions is likewise rooted in its use of structure. On Barsalou's account, this capacity to use structure (in the form of frames) must be innate.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  15
    Classes and collections: Principles of organization in the learning of hierarchical relations.Ellen M. Markman, Marjorie S. Horton & Alexander G. McLanahan - 1980 - Cognition 8 (3):227-241.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  17.  34
    Analogical inferences are central to analogy.Arthur B. Markman & Jeffrey P. Laux - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (4):390-391.
    It is important to take a developmental approach to the problem of analogy. One limitation of this approach, however, is that it does not deal with the complexity of making analogical inferences. There are a few key principles of analogical inference that are not well captured by the analogical relational priming (ARP) model.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  50
    Can developmental psychology provide a blueprint for the study of adult cognition?Arthur B. Markman - 2011 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34 (3):140-141.
    In order to develop sophisticated models of the core domains of knowledge that support complex cognitive processing in infants and children, developmental psychologists have mapped out the content of these knowledge domains. This research strategy may provide a blueprint for advancing research on adult cognitive processing. I illustrate this suggestion with examples from analogical reasoning and decision making.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  19.  37
    Choice output and choice processing: An analogy to similarity.Arthur B. Markman - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (3):423-424.
    The target article suggests that many practices of experimental economists are preferable to those used by psychologists studying judgment and decision making. The advantages of the psychological approach become clear when the focus of research shifts from choice output to choice processes. I illustrate this point with an example from research on similarity comparisons.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  42
    Ethical Difficulties with Randomized Clinical Trials Involving Cancer Patients: Examples from the Field of Gynecologic Oncology.Maurie Markman - 1992 - Journal of Clinical Ethics 3 (3):193-195.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  40
    Using regulatory focus to explore implicit and explicit processing in concept learning.Arthur Markman, W. Maddox & G. C. Baldwin - 2007 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 14 (9-10):132-155.
    Complex cognitive processes like concept learning involve a mixture of redundant explicit and implicit processes that are active simultaneously. This aspect of cognitive architecture creates difficulties in determining the influence of consciousness on processing. We propose that the interaction between an individual's regulatory focus and the reward structure of the current task influences the degree to which explicit processing is active. Thus, by manipulating people's motivational state and the nature of the task they perform, we can vary the influence of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  22. Deconstructing Self-Blame Following Sexual Assault: The Critical Roles of Cognitive Content and Process.Keith Markman, Audrey Miller, Ian Handley & Janel Miller - 2010 - Violence Against Women 16 (10):1120-1137.
    As part of a larger study, predictors of self-blame were investigated in a sample of 149 undergraduate sexual assault survivors. Each participant completed questionnaires regarding their preassault, peritraumatic, and post assault experiences and participated in an individual interview. Results confirmed the central hypothesis that, although several established correlates independently relate to self-blame, only cognitive content and process variables—negative self-cognitions and counterfactual-preventability cognitions—uniquely predict self-blame in a multivariate model.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  23.  52
    Language and the ability to evaluate contradictions and tautologies.Daniel N. Osherson & Ellen Markman - 1974 - Cognition 3 (3):213-226.
  24.  11
    Challenging the Requirements for Disclosing Risk to Clinical Research Participants.Maurie Markman - 2014 - American Journal of Bioethics 14 (4):11-12.
  25.  41
    A Tribute to Larry Erlbaum.Arthur B. Markman - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (1):1-1.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26. Effects of dichoptic viewing on bistable motion percepts.Ab Ritter & Bg Breitmeyer - 1987 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 25 (5):343-343.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  52
    Nonintentional similarity processing.Arthur B. Markman & Dedre Gentner - 2005 - In Ran R. Hassin, James S. Uleman & John A. Bargh (eds.), The New Unconscious. Oxford Series in Social Cognition and Social Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 107--137.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  28.  83
    Culture and individual differences.Arthur B. Markman, Serge Blok, John Dennis, Micah Goldwater, Kyungil Kim, Jeff Laux, Lisa Narvaez & Eric Taylor - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (6):831-831.
    Tests of economic theory often focus on choice outcomes and find significant individual differences in these outcomes. This variability may mask universal psychological processes that lead to different choices because of differences across cultures in the information people have available when making decisions. On this view, decision making research within and across cultures must focus on the processes underlying choice.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  29.  97
    Digging beneath rules and similarity.Arthur B. Markman, Sergey Blok, Kyungil Kim, Levi Larkey, Lisa R. Narvaez, C. Hunt Stilwell & Eric Taylor - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (1):29-30.
    Pothos suggests dispensing with the distinction between rules and similarity, without defining what is meant by either term. We agree that there are problems with the distinction between rules and similarity, but believe these will be solved only by exploring the representations and processes underlying cases purported to involve rules and similarity.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  30. A (Creative) Portrait of the Uncertain Individual: Self-Uncertainty and Individualism Enhance Creative Generation.Keith Markman, Kimberly Rios, Juliana Schroeder & Elizabeth Dyczewski - 2014 - Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 40 (8):1050-1062.
    Building on findings that self-uncertainty motivates attempts to restore certainty about the self, particularly in ways that highlight one’s distinctiveness from others, we show that self-uncertainty, relative to uncertainty in general, increases creative generation among individualists. In Studies 1 to 3, high (but not low) individualists performed better on a creative generation task after being primed with self-uncertainty as opposed to general uncertainty. In Study 4, this effect emerged only among those who were told that the task measured creative as (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  25
    Boundary conditions and the need for multiple forms of representation.Arthur B. Markman & Takashi Yamauchi - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (4):477-478.
    Multidimensional space representations like those posited in Edelman's target article are not sufficient to capture all similarity phenomena. We discuss phenomena that are compatible with models of similarity that assume structured relational representations. An adequate model of similarity and perception will require multiple approaches to representation.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  27
    Where are nature's joints? Finding the mechanisms underlying categorization.Arthur B. Markman - 2010 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (2-3):220-221.
    Machery argues that concepts are too heterogeneous to be a natural kind. I argue that the book does not go far enough. Theories of concepts assume that the task of categorizing warrants a unique set of cognitive constructs. Instead, cognitive science must look across tasks to find a fundamental set of cognitive mechanisms.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  36
    “Girls Are as Good as Boys at Math” Implies That Boys Are Probably Better: A Study of Expressions of Gender Equality.Eleanor K. Chestnut & Ellen M. Markman - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (7):2229-2249.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  34. Discrete thoughts: Why cognition must use discrete representations.Eric Dietrich & Arthur B. Markman - 2003 - Mind and Language 18 (1):95-119.
    Advocates of dynamic systems have suggested that higher mental processes are based on continuous representations. In order to evaluate this claim, we first define the concept of representation, and rigorously distinguish between discrete representations and continuous representations. We also explore two important bases of representational content. Then, we present seven arguments that discrete representations are necessary for any system that must discriminate between two or more states. It follows that higher mental processes require discrete representations. We also argue that discrete (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  35.  35
    Constructional sources of implicit agents in sentence comprehension.Micah B. Goldwater & Arthur B. Markman - 2006 - Cognitive Linguistics 20 (4).
  36.  42
    Categories and induction in young children.Susan A. Gelman & Ellen M. Markman - 1986 - Cognition 23 (3):183-209.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   220 citations  
  37.  19
    Editorial Statement.Arthur B. Markman - 2006 - Cognitive Science 30 (1):1-2.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  58
    The Interplay between Counterfactual Reasoning and Feedback Dynamics in Producing Inferences about the Self.Keith D. Markman, Ronald A. Elizaga, Jennifer J. Ratcliff & Matthew N. McMullen - 2007 - Thinking and Reasoning 13 (2):188 – 206.
    Counterfactual reasoning research typically demonstrates contrast effects—nearly winning evokes frustration, whereas nearly losing evokes exhilaration. The present work, however, describes conditions under which assimilative responses (i.e., when judgements are pulled towards a comparison standard) also occur. Participants solved analogies and learned that they had either nearly attained a target score or nearly failed to attain it. Participants in the no trajectory condition received this feedback in the absence of any prior feedback, whereas those in the trajectory condition received feedback after (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  39.  35
    The Research Misconception.Maurie Markman - 2004 - International Journal of Applied Philosophy 18 (2):241-252.
    Recently, several researchers and philosophers argued that clinical research trials are not therapy. Their position is based on foundational research ethics documents, such as the Belmont Report, on conceptual analysis, and on the general way clinical trials are conducted. After examining and rejecting these arguments, we claim that good research is consistent with good therapy; that often trials are good therapy; and that a blanket attack on clinical trials as non-therapeutic creates a research misconception. This misconception is potentially harmful because (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  40. Inference Using Categories.Takashi Yamauchi & Arthur B. Markman - 2000 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 26:776-795.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  41.  50
    Are dynamical systems the answer?Arthur B. Markman - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (1):50-51.
    The proposed model is put forward as a template for the dynamical systems approach to embodied cognition. In order to extend this view to cognitive processing in general, however, two limitations must be overcome. First, it must be demonstrated that sensorimotor coordination of the type evident in the A-not-B error is typical of other aspects of cognition. Second, the explanatory utility of dynamical systems models must be clarified.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  42.  59
    Counterfactuals Need Not be Comparative: The Case of “As If”.Keith D. Markman & Matthew N. McMullen - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (5-6):461-462.
    Byrne (2005) assumes that counterfactual thinking requires a comparison of facts with an imagined alternative. In our view, however, this assumption is unnecessarily restrictive. We argue that individuals do not necessarily engage in counterfactual simulations exclusively to evaluate factual reality. Instead, comparative evaluation is often suspended in favor of experiencing the counterfactual simulation as if it were real.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  15
    Problems of logic and evidence.Ellen M. Markman - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (2):194-195.
  44. Subject Index to Volume 30.Arthur B. Markman, Thomas T. Hills, Michael P. Kaschak, Jenny R. Saffran, Jarrod Moss, Kenneth Kotovsky, Jonathan Cagan, Louise Connell, Mark T. Keane & Joyca Pw Lacroix - 2006 - Cognitive Science 30:1129-1132.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45. The bRaIn.Arthur B. Markman - 2009 - In Sarah Robins, John Symons & Paco Calvo (eds.), The Routledge Companion to Philosophy of Psychology. New York, NY: Routledge. pp. 373.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  21
    The limitations of unification.Arthur B. Markman - 2007 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (1):33-34.
    There are two roadblocks to using game theory as a unified theory of the behavioral sciences. First, there may not be a single explanatory framework suitable for explaining psychological processing. Second, even if there is such a framework, game theory is too limited, because it focuses selectively on decision making to the exclusion of other crucial cognitive processes. (Published Online April 27 2007).
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47. Discourses on political violence: The problem of coherence.Ab Toit - 1990 - South African Journal of Philosophy 9 (4):191-213.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48. The Mental Simulation of Better and Worse Possible Worlds.Keith Markman, Igor Gavanski, Steven Sherman & Matthew McMullen - 1993 - Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 29 (1):87-109.
    Counterfactual thinking involves the imagination of non-factual alternatives to reality. We investigated the spontaneous generation of both upward counterfactuals, which improve on reality, and downward counterfactuals, which worsen reality. All subjects gained $5 playing a computer-simulated blackjack game. However, this outcome was framed to be perceived as either a win, a neutral event, or a loss. "Loss" frames produced more upward and fewer downward counterfactuals than did either "win" or "neutral" frames, but the overall prevalence of counterfactual thinking did not (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  49. A Reflection and Evaluation Model of Comparative Thinking.Keith Markman & Matthew McMullen - 2003 - Personality and Social Psychology Review 7 (3):244-267.
    This article reviews research on counterfactual, social, and temporal comparisons and proposes a Reflection and Evaluation Model (REM) as an organizing framework. At the heart of the model is the assertion that 2 psychologically distinct modes of mental simulation operate during comparative thinking: reflection, an experiential (“as if”) mode of thinking characterized by vividly simulating that information about the comparison standard is true of, or part of, the self; and evaluation, an evaluative mode of thinking characterized by the use of (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  50.  38
    The empirical case for role-governed categories.Micah B. Goldwater, Arthur B. Markman & C. Hunt Stilwell - 2011 - Cognition 118 (3):359-376.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
1 — 50 / 958