Results for 'Linguistic shortcut'

953 found
Order:
  1.  14
    The Linguistic Turn: Shortcut or Detour?Craig Dilworth - 1992 - Dialectica 46 (3‐4):201-214.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  28
    Linguistic Distributional Knowledge and Sensorimotor Grounding both Contribute to Semantic Category Production.Briony Banks, Cai Wingfield & Louise Connell - 2021 - Cognitive Science 45 (10):e13055.
    The human conceptual system comprises simulated information of sensorimotor experience and linguistic distributional information of how words are used in language. Moreover, the linguistic shortcut hypothesis predicts that people will use computationally cheaper linguistic distributional information where it is sufficient to inform a task response. In a pre‐registered category production study, we asked participants to verbally name members of concrete and abstract categories and tested whether performance could be predicted by a novel measure of sensorimotor similarity (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  3.  77
    Shortcuts and dynamic marking in the tableau method for adaptive logics.Diderik Batens & Joke Meheus - 2001 - Studia Logica 69 (2):221-248.
    Adaptive logics typically pertain to reasoning procedures for which there is no positive test. In [7], we presented a tableau method for two inconsistency-adaptive logics. In the present paper, we describe these methods and present several ways to increase their efficiency. This culminates in a dynamic marking procedure that indicates which branches have to be extended first, and thus guides one towards a decision — the conclusion follows or does not follow — in a very economical way.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  4. Slurs as the Shortcut of Discrimination.Bianca Cepollaro - 2017 - Rivista di Estetica 64:53-65.
    The last decade saw a growing interest for hate speech and the ways in which language reflects and perpetuates discrimination, with two main focuses of interest: a linguistic-oriented question about how slurs encode evaluation on the one hand, and a philosophical and psychological question about the effects elicited by slurs. In this paper, I show how the two questions are deeply related by illustrating how a certain linguistic analysis of derogatory epithets – the presuppositional one – can shed (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  5.  36
    (1 other version)Metaphor and mental shortcuts.Elly Ifantidou & Anna Piata - 2021 - Pragmatics and Cognition 28 (2):299-320.
    Cognitive-pragmatic approaches to how metaphors are understood view the activation of perceptual or motor effects as inferred. Crucially, inferences elicit conceptual representations, e.g. in the form of implicatures, and/or mental simulations, e.g. in the form of imagery, memory, an impression and other private elements. Emotional effects, being non-conceptual, must be left out of this picture. But evidence in neurolinguistics and psycholinguistics has shown that metaphors activate brain regions linked to emotions, and that in L2, in the absence of fully-propositional meaning, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  6.  74
    Principles of Representation: Why You Can't Represent the Same Concept Twice.Louise Connell & Dermot Lynott - 2014 - Topics in Cognitive Science 6 (3):390-406.
    As embodied theories of cognition are increasingly formalized and tested, care must be taken to make informed assumptions regarding the nature of concepts and representations. In this study, we outline three reasons why one cannot, in effect, represent the same concept twice. First, online perception affects offline representation: Current representational content depends on how ongoing demands direct attention to modality-specific systems. Second, language is a fundamental facilitator of offline representation: Bootstrapping and shortcuts within the computationally cheaper linguistic system continuously (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  7.  13
    Raising the Roof: Situating Verbs in Symbolic and Embodied Language Processing.John Hollander & Andrew Olney - 2024 - Cognitive Science 48 (4):e13442.
    Recent investigations on how people derive meaning from language have focused on task‐dependent shifts between two cognitive systems. The symbolic (amodal) system represents meaning as the statistical relationships between words. The embodied (modal) system represents meaning through neurocognitive simulation of perceptual or sensorimotor systems associated with a word's referent. A primary finding of literature in this field is that the embodied system is only dominant when a task necessitates it, but in certain paradigms, this has only been demonstrated using nouns (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  92
    The Paradigms of Biology.Marcello Barbieri - 2013 - Biosemiotics 6 (1):33-59.
    Today there are two major theoretical frameworks in biology. One is the ‘chemical paradigm’, the idea that life is an extremely complex form of chemistry. The other is the ‘information paradigm’, the view that life is not just ‘chemistry’ but ‘chemistry-plus-information’. This implies the existence of a fundamental difference between information and chemistry, a conclusion that is strongly supported by the fact that information and information-based-processes like heredity and natural selection simply do not exist in the world of chemistry. Against (...)
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  9. Is There a European Philosophy Science?: A Wake-up Call.Gereon Wolters - 2014 - Vienna Circle Institute Yearbook 17:277-293.
    The short answer to this question is a firm and unambiguous “yes and no”. The long answer will take the whole talk. Indeed, it could easily take an entire book. It is therefore unavoidable to take recourse here and there to simplifying shortcuts and polemical exaggerations, in order to get the message clear.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  10. Jay F. Rosenberg.Linguistic Roles & Proper Names - 1978 - In Joseph C. Pitt (ed.), The Philosophy of Wilfrid Sellars: Queries and Extensions: Papers Deriving from and Related to a Workshop on the Philosophy of Wilfrid Sellars held at Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University 1976. D. Reidel. pp. 12--189.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  11. Kendall L. Walton.Linguistic Relativity - 1973 - In Glenn Pearce & Patrick Maynard (eds.), Conceptual change. Boston,: D. Reidel. pp. 52--1.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12. Ferdinand de saussure.Linguistic Structuralism - 2010 - In Alan D. Schrift (ed.), The History of Continental Philosophy. London: Routledge. pp. 4--221.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13. Marshall Durbin and Michael Micklin.Contributions From Linguistics - forthcoming - Foundations of Language.
  14. Ian I-iacking.Linguistically Invariant Inductive Logic - 1970 - In Paul Weingartner & Gerhard Zecha (eds.), Induction, physics, and ethics. Dordrecht,: Reidel.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15. Derek Bickerton.Prolegomena to A. Linguistic - 1969 - Foundations of Language 5:34.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16. Ronald R. Butters.Dialect Variants & Linguistic Deviance - 1971 - Foundations of Language 7:239.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17. 4.1 Side Effects.Linguistic Side Effects - 2007 - In Chris Barker & Pauline I. Jacobson (eds.), Direct compositionality. New York: Oxford University Press.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18. N. Chomsky.Linguistic Competence - 1985 - In Jerrold J. Katz (ed.), The Philosophy of linguistics. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 80.
  19.  23
    The Other Languages of England.Malcolm Petyt & Linguistic Minorities Project - 1986 - British Journal of Educational Studies 34 (3):288.
  20. Linguistic behaviour.Jonathan Bennett - 1976 - New York: Cambridge University Press.
    First published in 1976, this book presents a view of language as a matter of systematic communicative behaviour.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   311 citations  
  21. On Being Bound to Linguistic Norms. Reply to Reinikainen and Kaluziński.Matthias Kiesselbach - 2020 - International Journal of Philosophical Studies 28 (4):1-14.
    The question whether a constitutive linguistic norm can be prescriptive is central to the debate on the normativity of meaning. Recently, the author has attempted to defend an affirmative answer, pointing to how speakers sporadically invoke constitutive linguistic norms in the service of linguistic calibration. Such invocations are clearly prescriptive. However, they are only appropriate if the invoked norms are applicable to the addressed speaker. But that can only be the case if the speaker herself generally accepts (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  22. Retweeting: its linguistic and epistemic value.Neri Marsili - 2021 - Synthese 198:10457–10483.
    This paper analyses the communicative and epistemic value of retweeting (and more generally of reposting content on social media). Against a naïve view, it argues that retweets are not acts of endorsement, motivating this diagnosis with linguistic data. Retweeting is instead modelled as a peculiar form of quotation, in which the reported content is indicated rather than reproduced. A relevance-theoretic account of the communicative import of retweeting is then developed, to spell out the complex mechanisms by which retweets achieve (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  23. Isaac Levi.Comments on‘Linguistically Invariant & Inductive Logic’by Ian Hacking - 1970 - In Paul Weingartner & Gerhard Zecha (eds.), Induction, physics, and ethics. Dordrecht,: Reidel.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24. Past the Linguistic Turn?Timothy Williamson - 2004 - In Brian Leiter (ed.), The future for philosophy. New York: Oxford University Press.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  25. Marfa-Luisa Rivero.Antecedents of Contemporary Logical & Linguistic Analyses in Scholastic Logic - 1973 - Foundations of Language 10:55.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26. Particular and general: Wittgenstein, linguistic rules, and context.Daniel Whiting - 2009 - In The later Wittgenstein on language. New York: Palgrave-Macmillan.
    Wittgenstein famously remarks that ‘the meaning of a word is its use’ (PI §43). Whether or not one views this as gesturing at a ‘theory’ of meaning, or instead as aiming primarily at dissuading us from certain misconceptions of language that are a source of puzzlement, it is clear that Wittgenstein held that for certain purposes the meaning of an expression could profitably be characterised as its use. Throughout his later writings, however, Wittgenstein’s appeal to the notion of use pulls (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  27. Linguistic authority and convention in a speech act analysis of pornography.Nellie Wieland - 2007 - Australasian Journal of Philosophy 85 (3):435 – 456.
    Recently, several philosophers have recast feminist arguments against pornography in terms of Speech Act Theory. In particular, they have considered the ways in which the illocutionary force of pornographic speech serves to set the conventions of sexual discourse while simultaneously silencing the speech of women, especially during unwanted sexual encounters. Yet, this raises serious questions as to how pornographers could (i) be authorities in the language game of sex, and (ii) set the conventions for sexual discourse - questions which these (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  28.  47
    Linguistic Privilege and Justice: What Can We Learn from STEM?Vitaly Pronskikh - 2018 - Philosophical Papers 47 (1):71-92.
    No categories
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  29. Escalating Linguistic Violence: From Microaggressions to Hate Speech.Emma McClure - 2019 - In Jeanine Weekes Schroer & Lauren Freeman (eds.), Microaggressions and Philosophy. New York: Taylor & Francis. pp. 121-145.
    At first glance, hate speech and microaggressions seem to have little overlap beyond being communicated verbally or in written form. Hate speech seems clearly macro-aggressive: an intentional, obviously harmful act lacking the ambiguity (and plausible deniability) of microaggressions. If we look back at historical discussions of hate speech, however, many of these assumed differences turn out to be points of similarity. The harmfulness of hate speech only became widely acknowledged after a concerted effort by critical race theorists, feminists, and other (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  30. Frege on Sense and Linguistic Meaning.Tyler Burge - 1990 - In David Bell & Neil Cooper (eds.), The Analytic Tradition: Roots and Scope. Blackwell. pp. 30-60.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  31. Cross-linguistic semantics.Maria Bittner - 1994 - Linguistics and Philosophy 17 (1):53 - 108.
    Rooth & Partee (1982) and Rooth (1985) have shown that the English-specific rule-by-rule system of PTQ can be factored out into function application plus two transformations for resolving type mismatch (type lifting and variable binding). Building on these insights, this article proposes a universal system for type-driven translation, by adding two more innovations: local type determination for gaps (generalizing Montague 1973) and a set of semantic filters (extending Cooper 1983). This system, dubbed Cross-Linguistic Semantics (XLS), is shown to account (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  32.  25
    Cross-Linguistic Word Recognition Development Among Chinese Children: A Multilevel Linear Mixed-Effects Modeling Approach.Connie Qun Guan & Scott H. Fraundorf - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    The effects of psycholinguistic variables on reading development are critical to the evaluation of theories about the reading system. Although we know that the development of reading depends on both individual differences (endogenous) and item-level effects (exogenous), developmental research has focused mostly on average-level performance, ignoring individual differences. We investigated how the development of word recognition in Chinese children in both Chinese and English is affected by (a) item-level, exogenous effects (word frequency, radical consistency, and curricular grade level); (b) subject-level, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  33.  49
    Principles for Object-Linguistic Consequence: from Logical to Irreflexive.Carlo Nicolai & Lorenzo Rossi - 2018 - Journal of Philosophical Logic 47 (3):549-577.
    We discuss the principles for a primitive, object-linguistic notion of consequence proposed by ) that yield a version of Curry’s paradox. We propose and study several strategies to weaken these principles and overcome paradox: all these strategies are based on the intuition that the object-linguistic consequence predicate internalizes whichever meta-linguistic notion of consequence we accept in the first place. To these solutions will correspond different conceptions of consequence. In one possible reading of these principles, they give rise (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  34.  51
    The linguistic interpretation of aphasic syndromes: Agrammatism in Broca's aphasia, an example.Mary-Louise Kean - 1977 - Cognition 5 (1):9-46.
  35.  58
    Non-linguistic strategies and the acquisition of word meanings.Eve V. Clark - 1973 - Cognition 2 (2):161-182.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  36. Derivation of Grammatical Sentences: Some Observations on Ancient Indian and.Modern Generative Linguistic Frameworks - 2000 - In Ajay K. Raina, B. N. Patnaik & Monima Chadha (eds.), Science and tradition. Shimla: Inter-University Centre for Humanities and Social Sciences, Indian Institute of Advanced Study.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37. Epistemic Contextualism and Linguistic Behavior.Wesley Buckwalter - 2017 - In Jonathan Jenkins Ichikawa (ed.), The Routledge Handbook of Epistemic Contextualism. New York: Routledge. pp. 44-56.
    Epistemic contextualism is the theory that “knows” is a context sensitive expression. As a linguistic theory, epistemic contextualism is motivated by claims about the linguistic behavior of competent speakers. This chapter reviews evidence in experimental cognitive science for epistemic contextualism in linguistic behavior. This research demonstrates that although some observations that are consistent with epistemic contextualism can be confirmed in linguistic practices, these observations are also equally well explained both by psychological features that do not provide (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  38. A mentalist framework for linguistic and extralinguistic communication.Bruno G. Bara & Maurizio Tirassa - 2010 - Linguistic and Philosophical Investigations 9:182-193.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  39.  40
    The Linguistic Idealism Question: Wittgenstein’s Method and his Rejection of Realism.Olli Lagerspetz - 2021 - Wittgenstein-Studien 12 (1):37-60.
    After the publication of Wittgenstein’s posthumous work the question was raised whether that work involved idealist tendencies. The debate also engaged Wittgenstein’s immediate students. Resistance to presumed idealist positions had been ideologically central to G.E. Moore, Bertrand Russell and other representatives of realism and early analytic philosophy. While Wittgenstein disagreed with them in key respects, he accepted their tendentious definition of ‘idealism’ at face value and bequeathed it to his students. The greatest flaw in the Realists’ view on idealism was (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  40. The Principles of Linguistic Philosophy.F. Waismann & R. Harré - 1965 - Foundations of Language 5 (1):128-134.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  41. Redundancy in Perceptual and Linguistic Experience: Comparing Feature-Based and Distributional Models of Semantic Representation.Brian Riordan & Michael N. Jones - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (2):303-345.
    Abstract Since their inception, distributional models of semantics have been criticized as inadequate cognitive theories of human semantic learning and representation. A principal challenge is that the representations derived by distributional models are purely symbolic and are not grounded in perception and action; this challenge has led many to favor feature-based models of semantic representation. We argue that the amount of perceptual and other semantic information that can be learned from purely distributional statistics has been underappreciated. We compare the representations (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  42.  38
    Chinese Grammar and the Linguistic Movement in Philosophy.Tsu-Lin Mei - 1961 - Review of Metaphysics 14 (3):463 - 492.
    There are in fact two questions to be discussed. Is the importance of a philosophical thesis relative to language? Is the validity of a philosophical thesis relative to language? The answer to both questions is "yes." It can be shown that two well-known philosophical theses--the logical distinction between numerals and adjectives drawn by Frege, and the distinction between tasks and achievements drawn by Ryle--are true but trivial when stated in Chinese. This is the program for the first part of this (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  43. The concept of a linguistic variable and its application to approximate reasoning.L. A. Zadeh - 1975 - Information Science 1.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  44.  21
    Apology strategies in Tashelhit: linguistic realization and religious influence.M’Hand Aatar, Hassan Skouri & Lalla Asmae Karama - 2024 - Lodz Papers in Pragmatics 20 (1):203-226.
    This study adopts the Cross-Cultural Speech Act Realization Patterns (CCSARP) framework to investigate the apology strategies used by L1 speakers of Tashelhit, a variety of Amazigh spoken in central Morocco. To this end, 82 university students either filled an assessment questionnaire or participated in an oral closed role-play. The findings indicated that L1 speakers of Tashelhit employed seven strategies to apologize, namely taking on responsibility, Illocutionary Force Indicating Devices (IFIDs), explanation or account, offer of repair, promise of forbearance, determinism, and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  58
    How to Fight Linguistic Injustice in Science: Equity Measures and Mitigating Agents.Aleksandra Vučković & Vlasta Sikimić - 2022 - Social Epistemology (1):1-17.
    Though a common language of science allows for easier communication of the results among researchers, the use of lingua franca also comes with the cost of losing some of the diverse ideas and results arising from the plurality of languages. Following Quine’s famous thesis about the indeterminacy of translation, we elaborate on the inherent loss of diverse ideas when only one language of science is used. Non-native speakers sometimes experience epistemic injustice due to their language proficiency and consequently, their scientific (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  46. Embodied cognition and linguistic comprehension.Daniel A. Weiskopf - 2010 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 41 (3):294-304.
    Traditionally, the language faculty was supposed to be a device that maps linguistic inputs to semantic or conceptual representations. These representations themselves were supposed to be distinct from the representations manipulated by the hearer’s perceptual and motor systems. Recently this view of language has been challenged by advocates of embodied cognition. Drawing on empirical studies of linguistic comprehension, they have proposed that the language faculty reuses the very representations and processes deployed in perceiving and acting. I review some (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  47.  9
    Side effects of the linguistic construction of others' wickedness.Encarnación Hidalgo Tenorio - 2010 - In Nancy Billias (ed.), Promoting and producing evil. New York: Rodopi. pp. 33.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48. The philosophical debate on linguistic bias: A critical perspective.Uwe Peters - 2024 - Philosophical Psychology 37 (6):1513-1538.
    Drawing on empirical findings, a number of philosophers have recently argued that people who use English as a foreign language may face a linguistic bias in academia in that they or their contributions may be perceived more negatively than warranted because of their English. I take a critical look at this argument. I first distinguish different phenomena that may be conceptualized as linguistic bias but that should be kept separate to avoid overgeneralizations. I then examine a range of (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  21
    A linguistic ontology of space for natural language processing.John A. Bateman, Joana Hois, Robert Ross & Thora Tenbrink - 2010 - Artificial Intelligence 174 (14):1027-1071.
  50. A Linguistic Turn in the Philosophy of Normativity?John Broome - 2015 - Analytic Philosophy 57 (1):1-14.
1 — 50 / 953