Results for ' classical aversive conditioning'

980 found
Order:
  1.  32
    Classical aversive conditioning of human digital volume-pulse change and tests of the preparatory-adaptive-response interpretation of reinforcement.John J. Furedy & Anthony N. Doob - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 89 (2):403.
  2.  21
    Stimulus-response contiguity in classical aversive conditioning.R. A. Champion - 1962 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 64 (1):35.
  3.  16
    Auditory and autonomic tests of the preparatory-adaptive-response interpretation of classical aversive conditioning.John J. Furedy - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 99 (2):280.
  4.  29
    Heart rate and somatic-motor coupling during classical aversive conditioning in humans.Paul A. Obrist - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (2):180.
  5.  18
    Contiguity and reinforcement in relation to CS-UCS intervals in classical aversive conditioning.Joan E. Jones - 1962 - Psychological Review 69 (3):176-185.
  6.  12
    S-R contiguity and delay of reinforcement as critical parameters in classical aversive conditioning.Ernest N. Damianopoulos - 1967 - Psychological Review 74 (5):420-427.
  7.  28
    Test of the preparatory adaptive response interpretation of aversive classical autonomic conditioning.John J. Furedy - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 84 (2):301.
  8.  70
    Classical conditioning: The new hegemony.Jaylan Sheila Turkkan - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):121-137.
    Converging data from different disciplines are showing the role of classical conditioning processes in the elaboration of human and animal behavior to be larger than previously supposed. Restricted views of classically conditioned responses as merely secretory, reflexive, or emotional are giving way to a broader conception that includes problem-solving, and other rule-governed behavior thought to be the exclusive province of either operant conditiońing or cognitive psychology. These new views have been accompanied by changes in the way conditioning (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   57 citations  
  9.  97
    Beyond potentiation: Synergistic conditioning in flavor-aversion learning. [REVIEW]W. Robert Batsell & Aaron G. Blankenship - 2002 - Brain and Mind 3 (3):383-408.
    Taste-aversion learning has been a popular paradigm for examining associative processes because it often produces outcomes that are different from those observed in other classical conditioning paradigms. One such outcome is taste-mediated odor potentiation in which aversion conditioning with a weak odor and a strong taste results in increased or synergistic conditioning to the odor. Because this strengthened odor aversion was not anticipated by formal models of learning, investigation of taste-mediated odor potentiation was a hot topic (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  10.  34
    Can human classical conditioning paradigms affect early facial processing? Modulation of the N170 and N250 in response to conditioning with aversive imagery and acoustic startle, findings from studies of healthy and depressed participants. [REVIEW]Camfield David, Kornfeld Emma, Mills Jessica & Croft Rodney - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  11.  15
    Backward conditioning and UCR latency.Marvin Homzie, Edward Rutledge & Milton Trapold - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (4):387.
  12.  40
    Unconfounded autonomic indexes of the aversiveness of signaled and unsignaled shocks.John J. Furedy & Felix Klajner - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 92 (3):313.
  13.  32
    Hume's Classical Theory of Justice.James King - 1981 - Hume Studies 7 (1):32-54.
    In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:32. HUME'S CLASSICAL THEORY OF JUSTICE1 Let me begin by formulating a broad distinction between two sorts of theories of justice. I shall stipulate that a modern theory of justice is one which treats justice as a moral quality, in fact as one moral quality among a multitude of moral virtues, and which accordingly takes the obligation tö' be just as pre-eminently a moral obligation. On this approach (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  14. Donald L. King.Classical Conditioning - 1983 - In Anees A. Sheikh (ed.), Imagery: Current Theory, Research, and Application. Wiley. pp. 156.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  3
    Aversive conditioning, anxiety, and the strategic control of attention.David S. Lee, Andrew Clement, Laurent Grégoire & Brian A. Anderson - forthcoming - Cognition and Emotion.
    What we pay attention to is influenced by both reward learning and aversive conditioning. Although early attention tends to be biased toward aversively conditioned stimuli, sustained ignoring of such stimuli is also possible. How aversive conditioning influences how a person chooses to search, or the strategic control of attention, has not been explored. In the present study, participants learned an association between a colour and an aversive outcome during a training phase, and in a subsequent (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  49
    How is justice understood in classic Confucianism?Christophe Duvert - 2018 - Asian Philosophy 28 (4):295-315.
    ABSTRACTIn Sinicized Asia, justice, conceptualized and institutionalized in its current form on a Western mold is part of a singular and ancient Confucian legal tradition.In this paper, it will be argued that Confucians initially articulated the concept of justice in relation to their own explanation of the world and their ideal, which distinguishes and rewards men’s actions according to their merits and social condition.It will be shown that Confucius’s thinking is primarily political and suggests ways of harmoniously organizing and reforming (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  17. Aversive-conditioning of naturally produced reward and nonreward odors in rats.Wr Batsell & H. Wayne Ludvigson - 1987 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 25 (5):333-333.
  18.  27
    Enhanced Instructed Fear Learning in Delusion-Proneness.Anaïs Louzolo, Rita Almeida, Marc Guitart-Masip, Malin Björnsdotter, Alexander Lebedev, Martin Ingvar, Andreas Olsson & Predrag Petrovic - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Psychosis is associated with distorted perceptions and deficient bottom-up learning such as classical fear conditioning. This has been interpreted as reflecting imprecise priors in low-level predictive coding systems. Paradoxically, overly strong beliefs, such as overvalued beliefs and delusions, are also present in psychosis-associated states. In line with this, research has suggested that patients with psychosis and associated phenotypes rely more on high-order priors to interpret perceptual input. In this behavioural and fMRI study we studied two types of fear (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  16
    Differentiating aversive conditioning in bistable perception: Avoidance of a percept vs. salience of a stimulus.Gregor Wilbertz & Philipp Sterzer - 2018 - Consciousness and Cognition 61:38-48.
  20.  36
    Classical eyelid conditioning as a function of sustained and shifted interstimulus intervals.Harvey C. Ebel & William F. Prokasy - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (1):52.
  21. Classical logic, conditionals and “nonmonotonic” reasoning.Nicholas Allott & Hiroyuki Uchida - 2009 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (1):85-85.
    Reasoning with conditionals is often thought to be non-monotonic, but there is no incompatibility with classical logic, and no need to formalise inference itself as probabilistic. When the addition of a new premise leads to abandonment of a previously compelling conclusion reached by modus ponens, for example, this is generally because it is hard to think of a model in which the conditional and the new premise are true.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  22. One-trial aversive conditioning to contextual crues: effects of time of shock presentation on freezing during conditioning and testing.Jh Roald Maes & Jmh Vossen - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (5):403-406.
  23.  30
    One-trial aversive conditioning to contextual cues: Effects of time of shock presentation on freezing during conditioning and testing.J. H. Roald Maes & Jo M. H. Vossen - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (5):403-406.
  24.  27
    Classical appetitive conditioning of the gsr with cool air as ucs, and the roles of ucs onset and offset as reinforcers of the cr.John J. Furedy - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 75 (1):73.
  25.  31
    Commentary: Olfactory aversive conditioning during sleep reduces cigarette-smoking behavior.Nicola Cellini & Valentina Parma - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  26.  40
    Attenuation of taste-aversion conditioning in rats recovered from thiamine deficiency: Atropine vs. lithium toxicosis.S. P. Sparenborg, W. F. Buskist, H. L. Miller, D. E. Fleming & P. C. Duncan - 1981 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 17 (5):237-239.
  27. (1 other version)Classical GSR conditioning: An evolutionary perspective.Kenneth R. Burstein - 1977 - Behaviorism 5 (2):113-126.
  28.  26
    Classical EDR conditioning using a truly random control and subjects differing in electrodermal lability level.Mary V. Solanto & Edward S. Katkin - 1979 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 14 (1):49-52.
  29.  15
    Overshadowing not potentiation in taste aversion conditioning.Peter J. Mikulka, Elizabeth Pitts & Christine Philput - 1982 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 20 (2):101-104.
  30.  55
    Differential classical eyelid conditioning as a function of CS intensity, CS rise time, and interstimulus interval.Susan M. Wilcox & Leonard E. Ross - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (2):272.
  31.  18
    Nonlinear risks: a unified framework.Pablo Gutiérrez Cubillos & Roberto Pastén - 2022 - Theory and Decision 95 (1):11-32.
    We study the conditions under which increasing risk raises the optimal control variable when the budget constraint is nonlinear. In contrast to the case when the budget constraint is linear, nonlinearities alter the risk attitudes of the economic agent; that is, the agent’s risk behavior is driven by the interaction between the shape of the utility function and the shape of the budget constraint. This paper complements the classical literature with linear payoff functions in two important ways. First, we (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  26
    Is there sign-tracking in aversive conditioning?William D. Bartter & Fred A. Masterson - 1980 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 15 (2):87-89.
  33.  39
    Brain indices of nonconscious associative learning.Philip S. Wong, Edward Bernat, S. Bunce & H. Shevrin - 1997 - Consciousness and Cognition 6 (4):519-544.
    Using a classical conditioning technique, this study investigated whether nonconscious associative learning could be indexed by event-related brain activity . There were three phases. In a preconditioning baseline phase, pleasant and unpleasant facial schematics were presented in awareness . A conditioning phase followed, in which stimuli were presented outside awareness , with an unpleasant face linked to an aversive shock and a pleasant face not linked to a shock. The third, postconditioning phase, involved stimulus presentations in (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  34.  15
    Age differences in the outcome of long-delay taste-aversion conditioning in rats.James R. Misanin, Douglas L. Greider & Charles F. Hinderliter - 1988 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26 (3):258-260.
  35.  13
    Modification of preference in a concurrent schedule by aversive conditioning: An analog study.Vernon L. Quinsey & George W. Varney - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 7 (2):211-213.
  36.  27
    Associative history, not familiarity, determines strength of taste-aversion conditioning in thiamine-deficient rats.W. F. Buskist, H. L. Miller, D. E. Fleming & S. P. Sparenborg - 1981 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 17 (2):104-106.
  37.  33
    Appetitive classical autonomic conditioning with subject-selected cool-puff UCS.Kenneth C. Kleist & John J. Furedy - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (3):598.
  38.  29
    Box 1. Amygdala subsysstems in appetitive and aversive conditioning.P. C. Holland, M. Gallagher, Peter C. Holland & Michela Gallagher - 1999 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3 (2):65-73.
  39.  17
    Social Fear Conditioning Paradigm in Virtual Reality: Social vs. Electrical Aversive Conditioning.Jonas Reichenberger, Sonja Porsch, Jasmin Wittmann, Verena Zimmermann & Youssef Shiban - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  40.  17
    Interaction of ethanol and congener contents in aversion conditioning to alcoholic beverages in rats.Joseph J. Franchina - 1987 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 25 (1):9-12.
  41.  22
    Ontogenetic differences in expressed fear of context following aversive conditioning.Philipp J. Kraemer, Christopher K. Randall & Timothy J. Carbary - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (3):223-225.
  42.  28
    On materialism.Sebastiano Timpanaro - 1975 - Atlantic Highlands [N.J.]: Humanities Press.
    This polemical work presents to the English-speaking world one of the most original philosophical thinkers to have emerged within post-war Europe. Sebastiano Timpanaro is an Italian classical philologist by training, an author of scholarly studies on the nineteenth-century poet Leopardi, and a Marxist by conviction. With great force and wit, On Materialism sets itself against what it sees as the virtually universal tendency within western Marxism since the war, to dissociate historical materialism from biological or physical materialism. Whereas the (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  43.  34
    Yoked comparisons of instrumental and classical eyelid conditioning.John W. Moore & I. Gormezano - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (6):552.
  44.  39
    Effects of unconditioned stimulus intensity and schedules of 50% partial reinforcement in human classical eyelid conditioning.Dennis L. Foth & Willard N. Runquist - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 84 (2):244.
  45.  35
    Brain indices of nonconscious associative learning.Philip S. Wong, Edward Bernat, S. . Bunce & Shevrin . - 1997 - Consciousness and Cognition 6 (4):519-544.
    Using a classical conditioning technique, this study investigated whether nonconscious associative learning could be indexed by event-related brain activity . There were three phases. In a preconditioning baseline phase, pleasant and unpleasant facial schematics were presented in awareness . A conditioning phase followed, in which stimuli were presented outside awareness , with an unpleasant face linked to an aversive shock and a pleasant face not linked to a shock. The third, postconditioning phase, involved stimulus presentations in (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  46.  34
    Comparison of trace and delay classical eyelid conditioning as a function of interstimulus interval.Susan M. Ross & Leonard E. Ross - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 91 (1):165.
  47.  21
    Reversal of an instrumental discrimination by classical discriminative conditioning.Milton A. Trapold, Douglas M. Gross & George W. Lawton - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 78 (4p1):686.
  48.  76
    Preparedness and phobias: Specific evolved associations or a generalized expectancy bias?Graham C. L. Davey - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):289-297.
    Most phobias are focussed on a small number of fear-inducing stimuli (e.g., snakes, spiders). A review of the evidence supporting biological and cognitive explanations of this uneven distribution of phobias suggests that the readiness with which such stimuli become associated with aversive outcomes arises from biases in the processing of information about threatening stimuli rather than from phylogenetically based associative predispositions or “biological preparedness.” This cognitive bias, consisting of a heightened expectation of aversive outcomes following fear-relevant stimuli, generates (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  49.  51
    Long-Term Visuo-Gustatory Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning Potentiate Human Visual Evoked Potentials.Gert R. J. Christoffersen, Jakob L. Laugesen, Per Møller, Wender L. P. Bredie, Todd R. Schachtman, Christina Liljendahl & Ida Viemose - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
  50. The liver as a mediator in taste-aversion conditioning.Sr Ellins & C. Costantino - 1987 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 25 (5):334-335.
1 — 50 / 980