Results for ' Concept learning'

974 found
Order:
  1.  26
    Concept learning and probability matching.George Mandler, Philip A. Cowan & Cecile Gold - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (6):514.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  2.  41
    Verbal concept learning as a function of instructions and dominance level.E. B. Coleman - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (2):213.
  3.  25
    Concept Learning: A Geometrical Model.Peter G.?Rdenfors - 2001 - Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 101 (2):163 - 183.
    In contrast to symbolic or associationist representations, I advocate a third form of representing information that employs geometrical structures. I argue that this form is appropriate for modelling concept learning. By using the geometrical structures of what I call conceptual spaces, I define properties and concepts. A learning model that shows how properties and concepts can be learned in a simple but naturalistic way is then presented. I also discuss the advantages of the geometric approach over the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  4.  30
    Concept learning as a function of the conceptual rule and the availability of positive and negative instances.L. E. Bourne, Bruce R. Ekstrand & Bonnie Montgomery - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (3):538.
  5.  21
    Conjunctive concept learning as affected by prior relevance information and other informational variables.Lance A. Miller - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 103 (6):1220.
  6.  23
    Simple concept learning as a function of intralist generalization.Marian Hooper Baum - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 47 (2):89.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  19
    Concept learning and nonmonotonic reasoning.Peter Gärdenfors - 2005 - In Henri Cohen & Claire Lefebvre, Handbook of Categorization in Cognitive Science (Second Edition). pp. 977-999.
    Humans learn new concepts extremely fast. One or two examples of a new concept are often sufficient for us to grasp its meaning. Traditional theories of concept formation, such as symbolic or connectionist representations, have problems explaining the quick learning exhibited by humans. In contrast to these representations, I advocate a third form of representing categories, which employs geometric structures. I argue that this form is appropriate for modeling concept learning. By using the geometric structures (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8. Concept learning.Tom J. Palmeri & David Noelle - 2002 - In Michael A. Arbib, The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks, Second Edition. MIT Press. pp. 234--238.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  9
    VIII -Concept Learning: A Geometrical Model.Peter Gardenfors - 2001 - Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 101 (2):163-183.
  10.  21
    Phonological Concept Learning.Elliott Moreton, Joe Pater & Katya Pertsova - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (1):4-69.
    Linguistic and non-linguistic pattern learning have been studied separately, but we argue for a comparative approach. Analogous inductive problems arise in phonological and visual pattern learning. Evidence from three experiments shows that human learners can solve them in analogous ways, and that human performance in both cases can be captured by the same models. We test GMECCS, an implementation of the Configural Cue Model in a Maximum Entropy phonotactic-learning framework with a single free parameter, against the alternative (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  11.  20
    Latency-choice discrepancy in concept learning.Marvin Levine - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (1p1):1.
  12. Can Bootstrapping Explain Concept Learning?Jacob Beck - 2017 - Cognition 158 (C):110–121.
    Susan Carey's account of Quinean bootstrapping has been heavily criticized. While it purports to explain how important new concepts are learned, many commentators complain that it is unclear just what bootstrapping is supposed to be or how it is supposed to work. Others allege that bootstrapping falls prey to the circularity challenge: it cannot explain how new concepts are learned without presupposing that learners already have those very concepts. Drawing on discussions of concept learning from the philosophical literature, (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  13.  41
    Instance contiguity in disjunctive concept learning.Robert C. Haygood, Jean Sandlin, Delmar J. Yoder & David H. Dodd - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (3):605.
  14. Concept learning.Bradley C. Love - 2003 - In L. Nadel, Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Nature Publishing Group.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  32
    Hypothesis behavior in a concept-learning task with probabilistic feedback.Steven P. Rogers & Robert C. Haygood - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 76 (1p1):160.
  16.  10
    Concept learning in nonprimate mammals: in search of evidence.Stephen Eg Lea - 2010 - In Denis Mareschal, Paul Quinn & Stephen E. G. Lea, The Making of Human Concepts. Oxford University Press.
  17.  34
    Concept learning with differing sequences of instances.Kenneth H. Kurtz & Carl I. Hovland - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 51 (4):239.
  18.  45
    Extending bayesian concept learning to deal with representational complexity and adaptation.Michael D. Lee - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (4):685-686.
    While Tenenbaum and Griffiths impressively consolidate and extend Shepard's research in the areas of stimulus representation and generalization, there is a need for complexity measures to be developed to control the flexibility of their “hypothesis space” approach to representation. It may also be possible to extend their concept learning model to consider the fundamental issue of representational adaptation. [Tenenbaum & Griffiths].
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  19.  25
    Memory effects in concept learning.Earl B. Hunt - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 62 (6):598.
  20. Concept learning: A geometrical model.Peter Gärdenfors - 2001 - Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 101 (2):163–183.
    In contrast to symbolic or associationist representations, I advocate a third form of representing information that employs geometrical structures. I argue that this form is appropriate for modelling concept learning. By using the geometrical structures of what I call conceptual spaces, I define properties and concepts. A learning model that shows how properties and concepts can be learned in a simple but naturalistic way is then presented. I also discuss the advantages of the geometric approach over the (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  21.  26
    Concept learning as a function of availability of previously presented information.Lyle E. Bourne, Sidney Goldstein & William E. Link - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (5):439.
  22.  22
    Concept learning and verbal control under partial reinforcement and subsequent reversal or nonreversal shifts.Daniel C. O'Connell - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (2):144.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  37
    Concept learning in a probabilistic language-of-thought. How is it possible and what does it presuppose?Matteo Colombo - 2023 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46:e271.
    Where does a probabilistic language-of-thought (PLoT) come from? How can we learn new concepts based on probabilistic inferences operating on a PLoT? Here, I explore these questions, sketching a traditional circularity objection to LoT and canvassing various approaches to addressing it. I conclude that PLoT-based cognitive architectures can support genuine concept learning; but, currently, it is unclear that they enjoy more explanatory breadth in relation to concept learning than alternative architectures that do not posit any LoT.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  17
    On Logical Characterisation of Human Concept Learning based on Terminological Systems.Farshad Badie - 2018 - Logic and Logical Philosophy 27 (4):545-566.
    The central focus of this article is the epistemological assumption that knowledge could be generated based on human beings’ experiences and over their conceptions of the world. Logical characterisation of human inductive learning over their produced conceptions within terminological systems and providing a logical background for theorising over the Human Concept Learning Problem (HCLP) in terminological systems are the main contributions of this research. In order to make a linkage between ‘Logic’ and ‘Cognition’, Description Logics (DLs) will (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  22
    Verbal concept learning as a function of instructions and dominance level.Benton J. Underwood & Jack Richardson - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 51 (4):229.
  26.  33
    Temporal dynamics of task switching and abstract-concept learning in pigeons.Thomas A. Daniel, Robert G. Cook & Jeffrey S. Katz - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:158480.
    The current study examined whether pigeons could learn to use abstract concepts as the basis for conditionally switching behavior as a function of time. Using a mid-session reversal task, experienced pigeons were trained to switch from matching-to-sample (MTS) to non-matching-to-sample (NMTS) conditional discriminations within a session. One group had prior training with MTS, while the other had prior training with NMTS. Over training, stimulus set size was progressively doubled from 3 to 6 to 12 stimuli to promote abstract concept (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  14
    Concept Learning: Convexity Versus Connectedness.Igor Douven & Steven Verheyen - forthcoming - Erkenntnis:1-18.
    In the context of the conceptual spaces framework, it has been argued that a natural concept is represented by a convex region in a similarity space. The convexity requirement has been defended on grounds of cognitive economy: among other benefits, concepts represented by convex regions have been said to be easily learnable, or more easily than concepts represented by nonconvex regions. There is some evidence that concepts in use are represented by regions that are convex, or at least almost (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  39
    Abstract concept learning in the pigeon.Thomas Zentall & David Hogan - 1974 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 102 (3):393.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  29. Creating World through Concept Learning.Claudia Lenz - 2018 - In Helge Jordheim & Erling Sandmo, Conceptualizing the world: an exploration across disciplines. New York: Berghahn.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  43
    Using regulatory focus to explore implicit and explicit processing in concept learning.Arthur Markman, W. Maddox & G. C. Baldwin - 2007 - Journal of Consciousness Studies 14 (9-10):132-155.
    Complex cognitive processes like concept learning involve a mixture of redundant explicit and implicit processes that are active simultaneously. This aspect of cognitive architecture creates difficulties in determining the influence of consciousness on processing. We propose that the interaction between an individual's regulatory focus and the reward structure of the current task influences the degree to which explicit processing is active. Thus, by manipulating people's motivational state and the nature of the task they perform, we can vary the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  4
    Induced dimensional set and concept learning.Don Fernandez - 1973 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1 (4):261-263.
  32.  14
    Logical settings for concept-learning.Luc De Raedt - 1997 - Artificial Intelligence 95 (1):187-201.
  33.  20
    Anxiety and verbal concept learning.Ralph F. Dunn - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 76 (2p1):286.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  12
    Concept learning and heuristic classification in weak-theory domains.Bruce W. Porter, Ray Bareiss & Robert C. Holte - 1990 - Artificial Intelligence 45 (1-2):229-263.
  35.  13
    Driver Attribute Filling for Genes in Interaction Network via Modularity Subspace-Based Concept Learning from Small Samples.Fei Xie, Jianing Xi & Qun Duan - 2020 - Complexity 2020:1-12.
    The aberrations of a gene can influence it and the functions of its neighbour genes in gene interaction network, leading to the development of carcinogenesis of normal cells. In consideration of gene interaction network as a complex network, previous studies have made efforts on the driver attribute filling of genes via network properties of nodes and network propagation of mutations. However, there are still obstacles from problems of small size of cancer samples and the existence of drivers without property of (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  46
    A Rational Analysis of Rule‐Based Concept Learning.Noah D. Goodman, Joshua B. Tenenbaum, Jacob Feldman & Thomas L. Griffiths - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (1):108-154.
    This article proposes a new model of human concept learning that provides a rational analysis of learning feature‐based concepts. This model is built upon Bayesian inference for a grammatically structured hypothesis space—a concept language of logical rules. This article compares the model predictions to human generalization judgments in several well‐known category learning experiments, and finds good agreement for both average and individual participant generalizations. This article further investigates judgments for a broad set of 7‐feature concepts—a (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   69 citations  
  37.  24
    Relationship of component cues to hypotheses in conjunctive concept learning.Irwin D. Nahinsky, William C. Penrod & Frank L. Slaymaker - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 83 (2p1):351.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  22
    Effect of amount of pretraining with identical and dissimilar stimuli on concept learning.Richard D. Petre - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (3):472.
  39.  31
    Stimulus sequence and concept learning.Marvin H. Detambel & Lawrence M. Stolurow - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 51 (1):34.
  40. The development of temporal concepts: Learning to locate events in time.Teresa McCormack & Christoph Hoerl - 2017 - Timing and Time Perception 5 (3-4):297-327.
    A new model of the development of temporal concepts is described that assumes that there are substantial changes in how children think about time in the early years. It is argued that there is a shift from understanding time in an event-dependent way to an event-independent understanding of time. Early in development, very young children are unable to think about locations in time independently of the events that occur at those locations. It is only with development that children begin to (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  41.  17
    Effect of intertrial interval duration on component processes in concept learning.Herbert Wells - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 94 (1):49.
  42.  15
    CLIP: concept learning from inference patterns.Ken'ichi Yoshida & Hiroshi Motoda - 1995 - Artificial Intelligence 75 (1):63-92.
  43. (1 other version)Why Concept Learning is a Good Idea.Chris Thornton - 1996 - In Andy Clark & Peter Millican, Connectionism, Concepts, and Folk Psychology: The Legacy of Alan Turing, Volume 2. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  21
    Number and type of available instances in concept learning.Vladimir Pishkin & Aaron Wolfgang - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (1):5.
  45.  35
    Effects of some sequential manipulations of relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions on concept learning.Richard C. Anderson & John T. Guthrie - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (4):501.
  46.  20
    Perception and simulation during concept learning.Erik Weitnauer, Robert L. Goldstone & Helge Ritter - 2023 - Psychological Review 130 (5):1203-1238.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47.  29
    Nonreinforcements of perceptual and mediating-responses in concept learning.Howard H. Kendler & Margaret Woerner - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (6):591.
  48. Concept learning and categorization: Models.John K. Kruschke - 2003 - In L. Nadel, Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Nature Publishing Group.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  13
    Reception versus selection procedures in concept learning.Frank S. Murray & Robert E. Gregg - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 82 (3):571.
  50.  44
    Prior relevance and dimensional homogeneity of partially reinforced dimensions after nonreversal shifts in concept learning.Frederick D. Abraham & James C. Taylor - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 75 (2):276.
1 — 50 / 974