Results for 'space-number mapping'

981 found
Order:
  1. Number-space mapping in human infants.Elizabeth S. Spelke & William James Hall - unknown
    Mature representations of number are built on a core system of numerical representation that connects to spatial representations in the form of a ‘mental number line’. The core number system is functional in early infancy, but little is known about the origins of the mapping of numbers onto space. Here we show that preverbal infants transfer the discrimination of an ordered series of numerosities to the discrimination of an ordered series of line lengths. Moreover, infants (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  2.  46
    Spontaneous mapping of number and space in adults and young children.Maria-Dolores de Hevia & Elizabeth S. Spelke - 2009 - Cognition 110 (2):198-207.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  3.  47
    Spontaneous mapping of number and space in adults and young children.Elizabeth S. Spelke Maria Dolores de Hevia - 2009 - Cognition 110 (2):198.
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  4.  61
    Linear mapping of numbers onto space requires attention.Giovanni Anobile, Guido Marco Cicchini & David C. Burr - 2012 - Cognition 122 (3):454-459.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  5. The mapping of numbers on space : Evidence for a logarithmic Intuition.Véronique Izard, Pierre Pica, Elizabeth Spelke & Stanislas Dehaene - 2008 - Médecine/Science 24 (12):1014-1016.
    Des branches entières des mathématiques sont fondées sur des liens posés entre les nombres et l’espace : mesure de longueurs, définition de repères et de coordonnées, projection des nombres complexes sur le plan… Si les nombres complexes, comme l’utilisation de repères, sont apparus relativement récemment (vers le XVIIe siècle), la mesure des longueurs est en revanche un procédé très ancien, qui remonte au moins au 3e ou 4e millénaire av. J-C. Loin d’être fortuits, ces liens entre les nombres et l’espace (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  37
    Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) map number onto space.Caroline B. Drucker & Elizabeth M. Brannon - 2014 - Cognition 132 (1):57-67.
  7.  35
    Newborn chicks need no number tricks. Commentary: Number-space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans' mental number line.Samuel Shaki & Martin H. Fischer - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  8. Singing Numbers… in Cognitive Space — A Dual‐Task Study of the Link Between Pitch, Space, and Numbers.Martin H. Fischer, Marianna Riello, Bruno L. Giordano & Elena Rusconi - 2013 - Topics in Cognitive Science 5 (2):354-366.
    We assessed the automaticity of spatial-numerical and spatial-musical associations by testing their intentionality and load sensitivity in a dual-task paradigm. In separate sessions, 16 healthy adults performed magnitude and pitch comparisons on sung numbers with variable pitch. Stimuli and response alternatives were identical, but the relevant stimulus attribute (pitch or number) differed between tasks. Concomitant tasks required retention of either color or location information. Results show that spatial associations of both magnitude and pitch are load sensitive and that the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  9.  19
    Response: “Newborn chicks need no number tricks. Commentary: Number-space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans' mental number line”.Rosa Rugani, Giorgio Vallortigara, Konstantinos Priftis & Lucia Regolin - 2016 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10.
  10. Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures.Pierre Pica, Stanislas Dehaene, Elizabeth Spelke & Véronique Izard - 2008 - Science 320 (5880):1217-1220.
    The mapping of numbers onto space is fundamental to measurement and to mathematics. Is this mapping a cultural invention or a universal intuition shared by all humans regardless of culture and education? We probed number-space mappings in the Mundurucu, an Amazonian indigene group with a reduced numerical lexicon and little or no formal education. At all ages, the Mundurucu mapped symbolic and nonsymbolic numbers onto a logarithmic scale, whereas Western adults used linear mapping with (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   68 citations  
  11. Space and the language‐cognition interface.Anna Papafragou - unknown
    According to classical theories of language and cognition, human cognition is characterized by strong universal commonalities built around notions such as object, space, agency, number, time, and event (Clark, 1973; Miller & Johnson‐Laird, 1976). Languages select from this prelinguistic conceptual repertoire the concepts that become encoded in their lexical and grammatical stock. Language acquisition, on this view, is a mapping process in which the learner needs to figure out which sounds in the language spoken in the environment (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  55
    Mapping Ethics Education in Accounting Research: A Bibliometric Analysis.Tamara Poje & Maja Zaman Groff - 2022 - Journal of Business Ethics 179 (2):451-472.
    The attention being paid to ethics education in accounting has been increasing, especially after the corporate accounting scandals at the turn of the century. This paper provides a comprehensive overview of the existing research in the field of ethics education in accounting. To synthesize past research, a bibliometric analysis that references 134 primary studies is performed and three bibliometric methods are applied. First, we visualize the historical evolution of ethics education in accounting research through historiography. Second, we use bibliographic coupling (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  13. State space semantics and conceptual similarity: Reply to Churchland.Francisco Calvo Garzón - 2000 - Philosophical Psychology 13 (1):77-95.
    Jerry Fodor and Ernest Lepore [(1992) Holism: a shopper's guide, Oxford: Blackwell; (1996) in R. McCauley (Ed.) The Churchlands and their critics , Cambridge: Blackwell] have launched a powerful attack against Paul Churchland's connectionist theory of semantics--also known as state space semantics. In one part of their attack, Fodor and Lepore argue that the architectural and functional idiosyncrasies of connectionist networks preclude us from articulating a notion of conceptual similarity applicable to state space semantics. Aarre Laakso and Gary (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  14.  51
    Gödel axiom mappings in special relativity and quantum-electromagnetic theory.William M. Honig - 1976 - Foundations of Physics 6 (1):37-57.
    Exponential mappings into an imaginary space or number field for the axioms of a theory, which are in the form of propositional constants and variables, make possible: (a) an understanding of the meaning and differences between the Lorentz transformation constants, such that their product is still equal to one, but the axioms at each end of the transformations are logically inverse and separately consistent; (b) an interpretation of the psi function phase factor which is part of the axiomE=hf; (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  15.  51
    Mapping relational links between motor imagery, action observation, action-related language, and action execution.Helen O’Shea - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16:984053.
    Actions can be physically executed, observed, imagined, or simply thought about. Unifying mental processes, such as simulation, emulation, or predictive processing, are thought to underlie different action types, whether they are mental states, as in the case of motor imagery and action observation, or involve physical execution. While overlapping brain activity is typically observed across different actions which indicates commonalities, research interest is also concerned with investigating the distinct functional components of these action types. Unfortunately, untangling subtleties associated with the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16. Response to Nunez.Véronique Izard, Stanislas Dehaene, Pierre Pica & Elizabeth Spelke - 2008 - Science 312 (5803):1310.
    We agree with Nuñez that the Mundurucu do not master the formal propreties of number lines and logarithms, but as the term "intuition" implies, they spontaneously experience a logarithmic mapping of number to space as natural and "feeling right.".
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  17.  37
    Regular spaces versus computing with chaos.Cees van Leeuwen - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (4):482-484.
    The attempt to provide a faithful mapping from distal shape space to proximal state space in terms of a higher order relationship defined over proximal similarity space stumbles on the context sensitivity of higher order relationships. Proportional analogy problems using quadruples of figures illustrate that for a number of interesting perceptual problems, the number of relevant dimensions cannot be reduced.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18. The adoration of a map: Reflections on a genome metaphor.Hub Zwart - 2009 - Genomics, Society and Policy 5 (3):1-15.
    On June 26, 2000, President Clinton, together with Francis Collins and Craig Venter, solemnly announced, from the East Room of the White House, that the grand effort to sequence the human genome, the Human Genome Project (HGP), was rapidly nearing its completion. Symbolism abounded. The event was framed as a crucial marker in the history of both humanity and knowledge by explicitly connecting the completion of the HGP with a number of already acknowledged and established scientific highlights. Tensions abounded (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  19. Response to Comment on "log or linear? Distinct Intuitions on the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures".Stanislas Dehaene, Véronique Izard, Pierre Pica & Elizabeth Spelke - 2009 - Science 323 (5910):38.
    The performance of the Mundurucu on the number-space task may exemplify a general competence for drawing analogies between space and other linear dimensions, but Mundurucu participants spontaneously chose number when other dimensions were available. Response placement may not reflect the subjective scale for numbers, but Cantlon et al.'s proposal of a linear scale with scalar variability requires additional hypotheses that are problematic.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  20.  19
    Meridians: Engagement and collaboration in physical and virtual public space.Claire Leporati - 2011 - Colloquy 22:247-260.
    The use of collaborative online social media applications as tools of communication is increasing in contemporary society. Correspondingly, a number of contemporary artists are exploring online interaction in their material public art practice, as a new form of documentation, promotion and creative collaboration. Mapping and analysing these new forms of interaction provide a method to determine the scope of their contribution to new artistic knowledge. This paper argues that contemporary public art practice can be cognisant of both physical (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21. Burqas in Back Alleys: Street Art, hijab, and the Reterritorialization of Public Space.John A. Sweeney - 2011 - Continent 1 (4):253-278.
    continent. 1.4 (2011): 253—278. A Sense of French Politics Politics itself is not the exercise of power or struggle for power. Politics is first of all the configuration of a space as political, the framing of a specific sphere of experience, the setting of objects posed as "common" and of subjects to whom the capacity is recognized to designate these objects and discuss about them.(1) On April 14, 2011, France implemented its controversial ban of the niqab and burqa , (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  22.  24
    Mapping Forbidden Places and Places of the Forbidden in Early Modern London and Paris.Christine M. Petto - 2010 - Environment, Space, Place 2 (1):35-59.
    In seventeenth- and eighteenth-century London and Paris, growing numbers of poor alarmed notables and city officials who would come to view a policy of confinement as an appropriate social, economic, religious, and political solution. This work examines the motivations of patrons to support these institutions (called hospitals). In particular, this study looks at their support for the construction (or renovations) of chapels (e.g. chapel at La Salpêtrière and the chapel at the Lock Hospital) and their visitations to these hospitals. Vagrants, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  23.  20
    Moving in Semantic Space in Prodromal and Very Early Alzheimer's Disease: An Item-Level Characterization of the Semantic Fluency Task.Aino M. Saranpää, Sasa L. Kivisaari, Riitta Salmelin & Sabine Krumm - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    The semantic fluency task is a widely used clinical tool in the diagnostic process of Alzheimer's disease. The task requires efficient mapping of the semantic space to produce as many items as possible within a semantic category. We examined whether healthy volunteers and patients with early Alzheimer's disease take advantage of and travel in the semantic space differently. With focus on the animal fluency task, we sought to emulate the detailed structure of the multidimensional semantic space (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24.  11
    More linear than log? Non-symbolic number-line estimation in 3- to 5-year-old children.Maciej Haman & Katarzyna Patro - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    The number-line estimation task has become one of the most important methods in numerical cognition research. Originally applied as a direct measure of spatial number representation, it became also informative regarding various other aspects of number processing and associated strategies. However, most of this work and associated conclusions concerns processing numbers in a symbolic format, by school children and older subjects. Symbolic number system is formally taught and trained at school, and its basic mathematical properties can (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  59
    A definability result for compact complex spaces.Dale Radin - 2004 - Journal of Symbolic Logic 69 (1):241-254.
    A compact complex space X is viewed as a 1-st order structure by taking predicates for analytic subsets of X, X \times X, … as basic relations. Let f: X→ Y be a proper surjective holomorphic map between complex spaces and set Xy:=f-1(y). We show that the set Ak,d:={y∈ Y: the number of d-dimensional components of Xy is (...)
    Direct download (9 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  26.  36
    Selective covering properties of product spaces.Arnold W. Miller, Boaz Tsaban & Lyubomyr Zdomskyy - 2014 - Annals of Pure and Applied Logic 165 (5):1034-1057.
    We study the preservation of selective covering properties, including classic ones introduced by Menger, Hurewicz, Rothberger, Gerlits and Nagy, and others, under products with some major families of concentrated sets of reals.Our methods include the projection method introduced by the authors in an earlier work, as well as several new methods. Some special consequences of our main results are : Every product of a concentrated space with a Hurewicz S1S1 space satisfies S1S1. On the other hand, assuming the (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  27.  14
    Linear Spatial–Numeric Associations Aid Memory for Single Numbers.John Opfer, Dan Kim, Christopher J. Young & Francesca Marciani - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Memory for numbers improves with age. One source of this improvement may be learning linear spatial-numeric associations, but previous evidence for this hypothesis likely confounded memory span with quality of numerical magnitude representations and failed to distinguish spatial-numeric mappings from other numeric abilities, such as counting or number word-cardinality mapping. To obviate the influence of memory span on numerical memory, we examined 39 3- to 5-year-olds’ ability to recall one spontaneously produced number (1-20) after a delay, and (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  28.  98
    Exploratory analysis of concept and document spaces with connectionist networks.Dieter Merkl, Erich Schweighoffer & Werner Winiwarter - 1999 - Artificial Intelligence and Law 7 (2-3):185-209.
    Exploratory analysis is an area of increasing interest in the computational linguistics arena. Pragmatically speaking, exploratory analysis may be paraphrased as natural language processing by means of analyzing large corpora of text. Concerning the analysis, appropriate means are statistics, on the one hand, and artificial neural networks, on the other hand. As a challenging application area for exploratory analysis of text corpora we may certainly identify text databases, be it information retrieval or information filtering systems. With this paper we present (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  29.  33
    The Presence of Background Noise Extends the Competitor Space in Native and Non‐Native Spoken‐Word Recognition: Insights from Computational Modeling.Themis Karaminis, Florian Hintz & Odette Scharenborg - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (2):e13110.
    Oral communication often takes place in noisy environments, which challenge spoken-word recognition. Previous research has suggested that the presence of background noise extends the number of candidate words competing with the target word for recognition and that this extension affects the time course and accuracy of spoken-word recognition. In this study, we further investigated the temporal dynamics of competition processes in the presence of background noise, and how these vary in listeners with different language proficiency (i.e., native and non-native) (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  30.  63
    Aesthetic Experience, Transitional Objects and the Third Space: The Fusion of Audience and Aesthetic Objects in the Performing Arts.Ian Woodward & David Ellison - 2010 - Thesis Eleven 103 (1):45-53.
    Aesthetic experience has been relativized and marginalized by recent social and cultural theory. As less attention has been paid to understanding the nature of aesthetic experience than mapping the distributed social correlates of tastes, its transformative potential and capacity to animate actors’ imaginations and actions goes unexplored. In this paper we draw upon a large number of in-depth interviews with performing arts audiences around Australia to investigate the language and discourse used to describe aesthetic experiences. In particular, we (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  49
    How Deep Is Your SNARC? Interactions Between Numerical Magnitude, Response Hands, and Reachability in Peripersonal Space.Johannes Lohmann, Philipp A. Schroeder, Hans-Christoph Nuerk, Christian Plewnia & Martin V. Butz - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:344216.
    Spatial, physical, and semantic magnitude dimensions can influence action decisions in human cognitive processing and interact with each other. For example, in the SNARC effect, semantic numerical magnitude facilitates left-hand or right-hand responding dependent on the small or large magnitude of number symbols. SNARC-like interactions of numerical magnitudes with the radial spatial dimension (depth) were postulated from early on. Usually, the SNARC effect in any direction is investigated using fronto-parallel computer monitors for presentation of stimuli. In such 2D setups, (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  32.  56
    How space-number associations may be created in preliterate children: six distinct mechanisms.Hans-Christoph Nuerk, Katarzyna Patro, Ulrike Cress, Ulrike Schild, Claudia K. Friedrich & Silke M. Göbel - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:126810.
    The directionality of space-number association (SNA) is shaped by cultural experiences. It usually follows the culturally dominant reading direction. Smaller numbers are generally associated with the starting side for reading (left side in Western cultures), while larger numbers are associated with the right endpoint side. However, SNAs consistent with cultural reading directions are present before children can actually read and write. Therefore, these SNAs cannot only be shaped by the direction of children’s own reading/writing behavior. We propose six (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  33.  50
    Space, Number, and Geometry From Helmholtz to Cassirer.Francesca Biagioli - 2016 - Cham: Springer Verlag.
    This book offers a reconstruction of the debate on non-Euclidean geometry in neo-Kantianism between the second half of the nineteenth century and the first decades of the twentieth century. Kant famously characterized space and time as a priori forms of intuitions, which lie at the foundation of mathematical knowledge. The success of his philosophical account of space was due not least to the fact that Euclidean geometry was widely considered to be a model of certainty at his time. (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  34. Space, number and structure: A tale of two debates.Stewart Shapiro - 1996 - Philosophia Mathematica 4 (2):148-173.
    Around the turn of the century, Poincare and Hilbert each published an account of geometry that took the discipline to be an implicit definition of its concepts. The terms ‘point’, ‘line’, and ‘plane’ can be applied to any system of objects that satisfies the axioms. Each mathematician found spirited opposition from a different logicist—Russell against Poincare' and Frege against Hilbert— who maintained the dying view that geometry essentially concerns space or spatial intuition. The debates illustrate the emerging idea of (...)
    Direct download (10 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  35.  61
    Karma or Immortality: Can Religion Influence Space-Time Mappings?Heng Li & Yu Cao - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (3):1041-1056.
    People implicitly associate the “past” and “future” with “front” and “back” in their minds according to their cultural attitudes toward time. As the temporal focus hypothesis proposes, future-oriented people tend to think about time according to the future-in-front mapping, whereas past-oriented people tend to think about time according to the past-in-front mapping. Whereas previous studies have demonstrated that culture exerts an important influence on people's implicit spatializations of time, we focus specifically on religion, a prominent layer of culture, (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  36.  26
    Time Points: A Gestural Study of the Development of Space–Time Mappings.Patrick Burns, Teresa McCormack, Agnieszka J. Jaroslawska, Patrick A. O'Connor & Eugene M. Caruso - 2019 - Cognitive Science 43 (12):e12801.
    Human languages typically employ a variety of spatial metaphors for time (e.g., “I'm looking forward to the weekend”). The metaphorical grounding of time in space is also evident in gesture. The gestures that are performed when talking about time bolster the view that people sometimes think about regions of time as if they were locations in space. However, almost nothing is known about the development of metaphorical gestures for time, despite keen interest in the origins of space–time (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  37.  71
    Francesca Biagioli. Space, Number, and Geometry from Helmholtz to Cassirer. [REVIEW]Thomas Mormann - 2018 - Philosophia Mathematica (2).
    © The Authors [2017]. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected] book Space, Number, and Geometry from Helmholtz to Cassirer is a reworked version of Francesca Biagioli’s PhD thesis. It aims ‘[to offer] a reconstruction of the debate on non-Euclidean geometry in neo-Kantianism between the second half of the nineteenth century and the first decades of the twentieth century’. More precisely, Biagioli concentrates on how the Marburg school of neo-Kantianism dealt with what (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38. Cartography as the Narrative of Space: A Map Lover's Discourse.Leovino Ma Garcia - 2002 - Budhi: A Journal of Ideas and Culture 6 (2):25-36.
    No categories
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39. Francesca Biagioli: Space, Number, and Geometry from Helmholtz to Cassirer: Springer, Dordrecht, 2016, 239 pp, $109.99 (Hardcover), ISBN: 978-3-319-31777-9. [REVIEW]Lydia Patton - 2019 - Journal for General Philosophy of Science / Zeitschrift für Allgemeine Wissenschaftstheorie 50 (2):311-315.
    Francesca Biagioli’s Space, Number, and Geometry from Helmholtz to Cassirer is a substantial and pathbreaking contribution to the energetic and growing field of researchers delving into the physics, physiology, psychology, and mathematics of the nineteenth and twentieth centuries. The book provides a bracing and painstakingly researched re-appreciation of the work of Hermann von Helmholtz and Ernst Cassirer, and of their place in the tradition, and is worth study for that alone. The contributions of the book go far beyond (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  26
    Deleuze’s Cinema Books: Three Introductions to the Taxonomy of Images.David Deamer - 2016 - Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh University Press.
    Deleuze’s two Cinema books explore film through the creation of a series of philosophical concepts. Not only bewildering in number, Deleuze’s writing procedures mean his exegesis is both complex and elusive. -/- Three questions emerge: What are the underlying principles of the taxonomy? How many concepts are there, and what do they describe? How might each be used in engaging with a film? -/- This book is the first to fully respond to these three questions, unearthing the philosophies inspiring (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  41. On the Relation Between Gauge and Phase Symmetries.Gabriel Catren - 2014 - Foundations of Physics 44 (12):1317-1335.
    We propose a group-theoretical interpretation of the fact that the transition from classical to quantum mechanics entails a reduction in the number of observables needed to define a physical state and \ to \ or \ in the simplest case). We argue that, in analogy to gauge theories, such a reduction results from the action of a symmetry group. To do so, we propose a conceptual analysis of formal tools coming from symplectic geometry and group representation theory, notably Souriau’s (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  42.  30
    Surreal Ordered Exponential Fields.Philip Ehrlich & Elliot Kaplan - 2021 - Journal of Symbolic Logic 86 (3):1066-1115.
    In 2001, the algebraico-tree-theoretic simplicity hierarchical structure of J. H. Conway’s ordered field${\mathbf {No}}$of surreal numbers was brought to the fore by the first author and employed to provide necessary and sufficient conditions for an ordered field (ordered$K$-vector space) to be isomorphic to an initial subfield ($K$-subspace) of${\mathbf {No}}$, i.e. a subfield ($K$-subspace) of${\mathbf {No}}$that is an initial subtree of${\mathbf {No}}$. In this sequel, analogous results are established forordered exponential fields, making use of a slight generalization of Schmeling’s conception (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  43. Algebraic and Kripke Semantics for Substructural Logics.Chrysafis Hartonas - 1994 - Dissertation, Indiana University
    A systematic approach to the algebraic and Kripke semantics for logics with restricted structural rules, notably for logics on an underlying non-distributive lattice, is developed. We provide a new topological representation theorem for general lattices, using the filter space X. Our representation involves a galois connection on subsets of X, hence a closure operator $\Gamma$, and the image of the representation map is characterized as the collection of $\Gamma$-stable, compact-open subsets of the filter space . The original lattice (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  44.  45
    Experimental Evidence From Newborn Chicks Enriches Our Knowledge on Human Spatial–Numerical Associations.Rosa Rugani, Giorgio Vallortigara, Konstantinos Priftis & Lucia Regolin - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (8):2275-2279.
    Núñez and Fias raised concerns on whether our results demonstrate a linear number-space mapping. Patro and Nuerk urge caution on the use of animal models to understand the origin of the orientation of spatial–numerical association. Here, we discuss why both objections are unfounded.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45. Aboutness.Stephen Yablo - 2014 - Oxford: Princeton University Press.
    Aboutness has been studied from any number of angles. Brentano made it the defining feature of the mental. Phenomenologists try to pin down the aboutness-features of particular mental states. Materialists sometimes claim to have grounded aboutness in natural regularities. Attempts have even been made, in library science and information theory, to operationalize the notion. But it has played no real role in philosophical semantics. This is surprising; sentences have aboutness-properties if anything does. Aboutness is the first book to examine (...)
  46.  31
    Will Review for Points: The Unpaid Affective Labour of Placemaking for Google’s ‘Local Guides’.Luis F. Alvarez León & Alexander Tarr - 2019 - Feminist Review 123 (1):89-105.
    A growing number of people are relying on technologies like Google Maps not only to navigate and locate themselves in cartographic space but also to search, discover and evaluate urban places. While the spatial data that underlies such technology frequently appears as a combination of Google-created maps and locational information passively collected from mobile (GPS-enabled) devices, in this article we argue that for such systems to function as both useful tools for exploration for users and sources of revenue, (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47. The Garage (Take One).Sean Smith - 2013 - Continent 3 (2):70-87.
    This piece, included in the drift special issue of continent. , was created as one step in a thread of inquiry. While each of the contributions to drift stand on their own, the project was an attempt to follow a line of theoretical inquiry as it passed through time and the postal service(s) from October 2012 until May 2013. This issue hosts two threads: between space & place and between intention & attention . The editors recommend that to experience (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  26
    Ancient space beyond maps. Barker, bouzarovski, pelling, isaksen new worlds from old texts. Revisiting ancient space and place. Pp. XVIII + 385, figs, ills, maps. Oxford: Oxford university press, 2016. Cased, £85, us$135. Isbn: 978-0-19-966413-9. [REVIEW]John Brendan Knight - 2017 - The Classical Review 67 (1):253-255.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  49.  42
    Anywhere or not at all: philosophy of contemporary art.Peter Osborne - 2013 - New York: Verso.
    A new reading of the philosophy of contemporary art by the author of The Politics of Time Contemporary art is the object of inflated and widely divergent claims. But what kind of discourse can open it up effectively to critical analysis? Anywhere or Not at All is a major philosophical intervention in art theory that challenges the terms of established positions through a new approach at once philosophical, historical, social and art-critical. Developing the position that “contemporary art is postconceptual art,” (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  50.  49
    Space-to-time mappings and temporal concepts.Kevin Ezra Moore - 2006 - Cognitive Linguistics 17 (2):199–244.
    Most research on metaphors that construe time as motion (motion metaphors of time) has focused on the question of whether it is the times or the person experiencing them (ego) that moves. This paper focuses on the equally important distinction between metaphors that locate times relative to ego (the ego-based metaphors Moving Ego and Moving Time) and a metaphor that locates times relative to other times (sequence is relative position on a path). Rather than a single abstract target domain TIME, (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
1 — 50 / 981