Results for ' spatial directions'

981 found
Order:
  1.  68
    Spatial Directions, Anisotropy and Special Relativity.Marco Mamone Capria - 2011 - Foundations of Physics 41 (8):1375-1397.
    The concept of an objective spatial direction in special relativity is investigated and theories assuming light-speed isotropy while accepting the existence of a privileged spatial direction are classified, including so-called very special relativity. A natural generalization of the proper time principle is introduced which makes it possible to devise non-optical experimental tests of spatial isotropy. Several common misunderstandings in the relativistic literature concerning the role of spatial isotropy are clarified.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  2.  39
    Effect of practice on a Stroop-like spatial directions task.Ronald E. Shor, Richard P. Hatch, Laurel J. Hudson, David T. Landrigan & Howard J. Shaffer - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 94 (2):168.
  3.  45
    The planar mosaic fails to account for spatially directed action.Roberta L. Klatzky & Nicholas A. Giudice - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (5):554 - 555.
    Humans' spatial representations enable navigation and reaching to targets above the ground plane, even without direct perceptual support. Such abilities are inconsistent with an impoverished representation of the third dimension. Features that differentiate humans from most terrestrial animals, including raised eye height and arms dedicated to manipulation rather than locomotion, have led to robust metric representations of volumetric space.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  4.  53
    Semantic gradients and interference in naming color, spatial direction, and numerosity.Leslie A. Fox, Ronald E. Shor & Robert J. Steinman - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 91 (1):59.
  5.  24
    The Impact of Coding Levels of Magnitude and of Spatial-Direction on the Spatial–Numerical Association of Response Codes Effect of Negative Numbers.Xiaojin Zeng, Jian Zhang, Longnong Dai & Yun Pan - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Whether negative numbers have a fixed spatial–numerical association of response codes effect, and whether the spatial representation of negative numbers is associated with negative numbers’ absolute or signed values remains controversial. In this study, through three experiments, the coding level of the magnitude and the spatial-direction is manipulated. In the first experiment, participants are required to code the magnitude and spatial-direction explicitly by using a magnitude classification task. In the second experiment, participants are forced to code (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  59
    Spatial learning in the T-maze: the influence of direction, turn, and food location.Hugh C. Blodgett, Kenneth McCutchan & Ravenna Mathews - 1949 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 39 (6):800.
  7.  28
    Modeling Mental Spatial Reasoning About Cardinal Directions.Holger Schultheis, Sven Bertel & Thomas Barkowsky - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (8):1521-1561.
    This article presents research into human mental spatial reasoning with orientation knowledge. In particular, we look at reasoning problems about cardinal directions that possess multiple valid solutions , at human preferences for some of these solutions, and at representational and procedural factors that lead to such preferences. The article presents, first, a discussion of existing, related conceptual and computational approaches; second, results of empirical research into the solution preferences that human reasoners actually have; and, third, a novel computational (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  8.  78
    Direct evidence for a parietal-frontal pathway subserving spatial awareness in humans.Michel T. de Schotten, Marika Urbanski, Hugues Duffau, Emmanuelle Volle, Richard Lévy, Bruno Dubois & Paolo Bartolomeo - 2005 - Science 309 (5744):2226-2228.
  9.  36
    Spatialization in working memory is related to literacy and reading direction: Culture “literarily” directs our thoughts.Alessandro Guida, Ahmed M. Megreya, Magali Lavielle-Guida, Yvonnick Noël, Fabien Mathy, Jean-Philippe van Dijck & Elger Abrahamse - 2018 - Cognition 175 (C):96-100.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  10.  31
    Walking direction triggers visuo-spatial orienting in 6-month-old infants and adults: An eye tracking study.Lara Bardi, Elisa Di Giorgio, Marco Lunghi, Nikolaus F. Troje & Francesca Simion - 2015 - Cognition 141 (C):112-120.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  11.  26
    Assessing spatial frameworks with object and direction probes.David J. Bryant & Barbara Tversky - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (1):29-32.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  12.  33
    Directing spatial attention to locations within remembered and imagined mental representations.Simon G. Gosling & Duncan E. Astle - 2013 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7.
  13.  90
    Spatial Updating according to a Fixed Reference Direction of a Briefly Viewed Layout.Timothy P. McNamara Hui Zhang, Weimin Mou - 2011 - Cognition 119 (3):419.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  14.  32
    Studies in spatial learning. VI. Place orientation and direction orientation.Benbow F. Ritchie - 1948 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 38 (6):659.
  15.  22
    Direct Gaze Partially Overcomes Hemispatial Neglect and Captures Spatial Attention.Miguel Leal Rato, Inês Mares, Diana Aguiar de Sousa, Atsushi Senju & Isabel Pavão Martins - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  16.  24
    Short-term memory for spatial location in goal-directed locomotion.Digby Elliott, Ruth Jones & Susan Gray - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (2):158-160.
  17.  32
    Investigating the spatial characteristics of the crossmodal interaction between nociception and vision using gaze direction.Lieve Filbrich, Monika Halicka, Andrea Alamia & Valéry Legrain - 2018 - Consciousness and Cognition 57:106-115.
  18.  49
    Reference directions and reference objects in spatial memory of a briefly viewed layout.Weimin Mou, Chengli Xiao & Timothy P. McNamara - 2008 - Cognition 108 (1):136-154.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  19. Spatial correlation in directionally selective complex cells of cat area 17.R. J. A. van Wezel, M. J. M. Lankheet, S. O. Dumoulin & W. A. van de Grind - 1996 - In Enrique Villanueva, Perception. Ridgeview Pub. Co. pp. 125-126.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  28
    Commanding the direction of passive whole-body rotations facilitates egocentric spatial updating.Yves-André Féry, Richard Magnac & Isabelle Israël - 2004 - Cognition 91 (2):B1-B10.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  21.  79
    Isolating observer-based reference directions in human spatial memory: Head, body, and the self-to-array axis.Adam Richardson David Waller, Yvonne Lippa - 2008 - Cognition 106 (1):157.
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  22.  30
    The Joint Effects of Spatial Cueing and Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Visual Acuity.Taly Bonder, Daniel Gopher & Yaffa Yeshurun - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  23.  25
    Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Baseline and Slope of Prefrontal Cortex Hemodynamics During a Spatial Working Memory Task.Ryan McKendrick, Brian Falcone, Melissa Scheldrup & Hasan Ayaz - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
  24. Silence Perception and Spatial Content.Błażej Skrzypulec - 2022 - Australasian Journal of Philosophy 100 (3):524-538.
    It seems plausible that visual experiences of darkness have perceptual phenomenal content that clearly differentiates them from absences of visual experiences. I argue, relying on psychological results concerning auditory attention, that the analogous claim is true about auditory experiences of silence. More specifically, I propose that experiences of silence present empty spatial directions like ‘right’ or ‘left’, and so have egocentric spatial content. Furthermore, I claim that such content is genuinely auditory and phenomenal in the sense that (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  25.  67
    Stepping Into a Map: Initial Heading Direction Influences Spatial Memory Flexibility.Stephanie A. Gagnon, Tad T. Brunyé, Aaron Gardony, Matthijs L. Noordzij, Caroline R. Mahoney & Holly A. Taylor - 2014 - Cognitive Science 38 (2):275-302.
    Learning a novel environment involves integrating first-person perceptual and motoric experiences with developing knowledge about the overall structure of the surroundings. The present experiments provide insights into the parallel development of these egocentric and allocentric memories by intentionally conflicting body- and world-centered frames of reference during learning, and measuring outcomes via online and offline measures. Results of two experiments demonstrate faster learning and increased memory flexibility following route perspective reading (Experiment 1) and virtual navigation (Experiment 2) when participants begin exploring (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  26. Reconsidering 'spatial memory' and the Morris water maze.Jacqueline Anne Sullivan - 2010 - Synthese 177 (2):261-283.
    The Morris water maze has been put forward in the philosophy of neuroscience as an example of an experimental arrangement that may be used to delineate the cognitive faculty of spatial memory (e.g., Craver and Darden, Theory and method in the neurosciences, University of Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, 2001; Craver, Explaining the brain: Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2007). However, in the experimental and review literature on the water maze throughout the history of its (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  27. “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition.Barbara Landau & Ray Jackendoff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):217-238.
    Fundamental to spatial knowledge in all species are the representations underlying object recognition, object search, and navigation through space. But what sets humans apart from other species is our ability to express spatial experience through language. This target article explores the language ofobjectsandplaces, asking what geometric properties are preserved in the representations underlying object nouns and spatial prepositions in English. Evidence from these two aspects of language suggests there are significant differences in the geometric richness with which (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   104 citations  
  28.  63
    Response actions influence the categorization of directions in auditory space.Marcella C. C. Velten, Bettina E. Bläsing, Thomas Hermann, Constanze Vorwerg & Thomas Schack - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:147772.
    Spatial region concepts such as “front,” “back,” “left,” and “right” reflect our typical interaction with space, and the corresponding surrounding regions have different statuses in memory. We examined the representation of spatial directions in the auditory space, specifically in how far natural response actions, such as orientation movements toward a sound source, would affect the categorization of egocentric auditory space. While standing in the middle of a circle with 16 loudspeakers, participants were presented acoustic stimuli coming from (...)
    Direct download (8 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  29.  23
    Learned Spatial Schemas and Prospective Hippocampal Activity Support Navigation After One-Shot Learning.Marlieke T. R. van Kesteren, Thackery I. Brown & Anthony D. Wagner - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12:373355.
    Prior knowledge structures (or schemas) confer multiple behavioral benefits. First, when we encounter information that fits with prior knowledge structures, this information is generally better learned and remembered. Second, prior knowledge can support prospective planning. In humans, memory enhancements related to prior knowledge have been suggested to be supported, in part, by computations in prefrontal and medial temporal lobe cortex. Moreover, animal studies further implicate a role for the hippocampus in schema-based facilitation and in the emergence of prospective planning signals (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  30.  62
    Spatial Representations Elicit Dual‐Coding Effects in Mental Imagery.Michelle Verges & Sean Duffy - 2009 - Cognitive Science 33 (6):1157-1172.
    Spatial aspects of words are associated with their canonical locations in the real world. Yet little research has tested whether spatial associations denoted in language comprehension generalize to their corresponding images. We directly tested the spatial aspects of mental imagery in picture and word processing (Experiment 1). We also tested whether spatial representations of motion words produce similar perceptual-interference effects as demonstrated by object words (Experiment 2). Findings revealed that words denoting an upward spatial location (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  31.  72
    (1 other version)Keeping Track of Invisible Individuals While Exploring a Spatial Layout with Partial Cues: Location-based and Deictic Direction-based Strategies.Nicolas Bullot - 2008 - Philosophical Psychology 21 (1):15-46.
    In contrast to Constructivist Views, which construe perceptual cognition as an essentially reconstructive process, this article recommends the Deictic View, which grounds perception in perceptual-demonstrative reference and the use of deictic tracking strategies for acquiring and updating knowledge about individuals. The view raises the problem of how sensory-motor tracking connects to epistemic and integrated forms of tracking. To study the strategies used to solve this problem, we report a study of the ability to track distal individuals when only their (...) can be perceived and not their locations. We introduce a new experimental paradigm named the 'Modified Traveling Salesman Problem' (MTSP), which requires subjects to visit n invisible targets in a 2D display once each. Surprisingly, subjects are competent at this task for up to 10 targets. We consider two types of tracking strategies that subjects might use: 'location-based' strategies and 'deictic direction-based' strategies. A number of observations suggest that subjects used the latter, at least for larger numbers of targets. We hypothesize that subjects used perceptual-demonstrative reference and deictic strategies (i) to perform the sensory-motor tracking of directional segments, (ii) to bind the segments with their updated status in the task, and (iii) to perform the epistemic tracking of invisible targets by means of perception-based inferences. (shrink)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  32. Spatial aspects of olfactory experience.Solveig Aasen - 2018 - Canadian Journal of Philosophy 49 (8):1041-1061.
    Several theorists argue that one does not experience something as being at or coming from a distance or direction in olfaction. In contrast to this, I suggest that there can be a variety of spatial aspects of both synchronic and diachronic olfactory experiences, including spatial distance and direction. I emphasise, however, that these are not aspects of every olfactory experience. Thus, I suggest renouncing the widespread assumption there is a uniform account of the nature, including the spatial (...)
    Direct download (12 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  33.  41
    Samuel Merrill III and Bernard Grofman, A Unified Theory of Voting: Directional and Proximity Spatial Models, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999.Hiroshi Hirano - 2001 - Japanese Journal of Political Science 2 (2):257-271.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34. Visual matching of spatial intervals and visually directed walking-a comparison.J. M. Loomis, J. A. DaSilva & S. L. Marques - 1987 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 25 (5):344-344.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  35.  57
    A spatially oriented decision does not induce consciousness in a motor task.Bruce Bridgeman & Valerie Huemer - 1998 - Consciousness and Cognition 7 (3):454-464.
    Visual information follows at least two branches in the human nervous system, following a common input stage: a cognitive ''what'' branch governs perception and experience, while a sensorimotor ''how'' branch handles visually guided behavior though its outputs are unconscious. The sensorimotor system is probed with an isomorphic task, requiring a 1:1 relationship between target position and motor response. The cognitive system, in contrast, is probed with a forced qualitative decision, expressed verbally, about the location of a target. Normally, the cognitive (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  36. The Spatial Content of Experience.Brad Thompson - 2010 - Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 81 (1):146-184.
    To what extent is the external world the way that it appears to us in perceptual experience? This perennial question in philosophy is no doubt ambiguous in many ways. For example, it might be taken as equivalent to the question of whether or not the external world is the way that it appears to be? This is a question about the epistemology of perception: Are our perceptual experiences by and large veridical representations of the external world? Alternatively, the question might (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  37.  25
    Spatial Alignment Facilitates Visual Comparison in Children.Yinyuan Zheng, Bryan Matlen & Dedre Gentner - 2022 - Cognitive Science 46 (8):e13182.
    Visual comparison is a key process in everyday learning and reasoning. Recent research has discovered the spatial alignment principle, based on the broader framework of structure‐mapping theory in comparison. According to the spatial alignment principle, visual comparison is more efficient when the figures being compared are arranged in direct placement—that is, juxtaposed with parallel structural axes. In this placement, (1) the intended relational correspondences are readily apparent, and (2) the influence of potential competing correspondences is minimized. There is (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  38.  19
    Spatial cognition and the avian hippocampus: Research in domestic chicks.Anastasia Morandi-Raikova & Uwe Mayer - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    In this review, we discuss the functional equivalence of the avian and mammalian hippocampus, based mostly on our own research in domestic chicks, which provide an important developmental model. In birds, like in mammals, the hippocampus plays a central role in processing spatial information. However, the structure of this homolog area shows remarkable differences between birds and mammals. To understand the evolutionary origin of the neural mechanisms for spatial navigation, it is important to test how far theories developed (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  39.  52
    Spatial Models Design Reasons and the Construction of Spatial Meaning.John Peponis, Iris Lycourioti & Iphigenia Mari - 2002 - Philosophica 70 (2).
    Based on architectural projects which interpret literature as program we discuss design reasoning when no routine models of problem solving apply. We address three aspects of formulation: defining the design charge so that it can be retrospectively stated independent of the actual proposal; defining a language of formal operations; and defining the intrinsic aims of design that are only intimated through the proposal itself. The coherence of the project is a function of the way in which formal properties interact, and (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40. Spatial Entities.Roberto Casati & Achille C. Varzi - 1997 - In Oliviero Stock, Spatial and Temporal Reasoning. Kluwer Academic Publishers. pp. 73–96.
    Ordinary reasoning about space—we argue—is first and foremost reasoning about things or events located in space. Accordingly, any theory concerned with the construction of a general model of our spatial competence must be grounded on a general account of the sort of entities that may enter into the scope of the theory. Moreover, on the methodological side the emphasis on spatial entities (as opposed to purely geometrical items such as points or regions) calls for a reexamination of the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  41.  62
    Co-ordination of spatial perspectives in response to addressee feedback.Kavita E. Thomas & Elena Andonova - 2012 - Pragmatics and Cognition 20 (3):505-545.
    In this paper we investigate the effect of level of understanding revealed by feedback in the form of clarification requests from a route follower on a route giver’s spatial perspective choice in their response in route instruction dialogues. In an experiment varying the level of understanding displayed by route follower clarification requests, route giver perspective switching in response to this feedback is investigated. Three levels of understanding displayed by feedback are investigated: low-level clarification requests indicating that the instruction was (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42. Bodily spatial content.Frederique de Vignemont - 2009 - PSYCHE: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research On Consciousness 15 (1).
    The classic notion of an egocentric frame of reference cannot be easily applied to bodily space, given the difficulties in providing a centre of such frame as well as axes on which one could compute distances and directions . Yet, Smith tries to rehabilitate the egocentric account of bodily frame by switching from an anatomical definition of egocentricity to a more functional definition . Here I will review some empirical evidence that shows that one cannot ground bodily experiences in (...)
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  43.  35
    Innate and Cultural Spatial Time: A Developmental Perspective.Barbara Magnani & Alessandro Musetti - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
    We reviewed literature to understand when a spatial map for time is available in the brain. We carefully defined the concepts of metrical map of time and of conceptual representation of time as the mental time line (MTL) in order to formulate our position. It is that both metrical map and conceptual representation of time are spatial in nature. The former should be innate, related to motor/implicit timing, it should represent all magnitudes with an analogic and bi-dimensional structure. (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  44.  47
    Evaluating Spatial Interpolation Maps of the Age Structure of the Population of Thi Qar Governorate Using Geographic Information Systems (GIS) Technologies.Nariman Jamal Kazem & Wissam Ahmed Rashid - forthcoming - Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture.
    The research aims to harness spatial interpolation techniques to produce maps with a high level of perceptual accuracy in representing the population data of the study area. This is achieved after exploring the statistical and spatial nature of the databases used, analyzing them, and determining their distribution using a variety of spatial data exploration tools available within the GIS (Geographic Information Systems) environment. These tools contribute to evaluating the characteristics, distribution, and analysis of data, including testing data (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  45.  43
    Spatial Association and Explanation of China’s Digital Financial Inclusion Development Based on the Network Analysis Method.Xiaojie Liu, Jiannan Zhu, Jianfeng Guo & Changnan Cui - 2021 - Complexity 2021:1-13.
    As an essential direction for developing inclusive finance, digital financial inclusion breaks through the time and space constraints of inclusive finance development and has extensive connections between different regions. However, no research has modelled the network connections and the role and position of different digital financial inclusion development regions. This study constructed the spatial association network of China’s digital financial inclusion development and used the network analysis method and the quadratic assignment procedure method to study the structural and locational (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  46.  33
    Spin and Wind Directions I: Identifying Entanglement in Nature and Cognition.Robrecht Vanderbeeken & Frederik Le Roy - 2018 - Foundations of Science 23 (2):323-335.
    We present a cognitive psychology experiment where participants were asked to select pairs of spatial directions that they considered to be the best example of Two different wind directions. Data are shown to violate the CHSH version of Bell’s inequality with the same magnitude as in typical Bell-test experiments with entangled spins. Wind directions thus appear to be conceptual entities connected through meaning, in human cognition, in a similar way as spins appear to be entangled in (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  47. Directional Equilibria.Hun Chung & John Duggan - 2018 - Journal of Theoretical Politics 30 (3):272-305.
    We propose the solution concept of directional equilibrium for the multidimensional model of voting with general spatial preferences. This concept isolates alternatives that are stable with respect to forces applied by all voters in the directions of their gradients, and it extends a known concept from statistics for Euclidean preferences. We establish connections to the majority core, Pareto optimality, and existence and closed graph, and we provide non-cooperative foundations in terms of a local contest game played by voters.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  48.  80
    Spatial Network Structures of Urban Agglomeration Based on the Improved Gravity Model: A Case Study in China’s Two Urban Agglomerations.Yubo Zhao, Gui Zhang & Hongwei Zhao - 2021 - Complexity 2021:1-17.
    Research on urban agglomerations from the perspective of network spatial structure is important to promote their sustainable development. Based on online and traditional data, this paper first improves three aspects of the traditional spatial gravity model—city quality, the gravitation coefficient, and city distance—considering urban center functional intensity and population mobility tendencies. The resulting improved directional gravity model is applied to analyze the structure of the city network for two urban agglomerations in China, the Beijing-Tianjin-Hebei urban agglomeration and the (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  49.  69
    Novel-view Scene Recognition Relies on Identifying Spatial Reference Directions.Timothy P. McNamara Weimin Mou, Hui Zhang - 2009 - Cognition 111 (2):175.
  50.  22
    Spatial and Motor Aspects in the “Action-Sentence Compatibility Effect”.Alberto Greco - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    The Action-sentence Compatibility Effect is often taken as supporting the fundamental role of the motor system in understanding sentences that describe actions. This effect would be related to an internal “simulation,” i.e., the reactivation of past perceptual and motor experiences. However, it is not easy to establish whether this simulation predominantly involves spatial imagery or motor anticipation. In the classical ACE experiments, where a real motor response is required, the direction and motor representations are mixed. In order to disentangle (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 981