Results for ' kinesthetic stimulus'

992 found
Order:
  1.  34
    Kinesthetic aftereffect and mode of exposure to the inspection stimulus.Paul Bakan & Ernest Weiler - 1963 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 65 (3):319.
  2.  35
    Reaction time to kinesthetic stimulation resulting from sudden arm displacement.Rube Chernikoff & Franklin V. Taylor - 1952 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 43 (1):1.
  3.  40
    Supplementary report: Directional effects and sex in kinesthetic aftereffects.Paul Bakan, Richard Thompson & Gail Wildes - 1961 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (6):509.
  4.  24
    Spatial S-R compatibility effects involving kinesthetic cues.Richard J. Wallace - 1972 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 93 (1):163.
  5.  42
    Conditioning of the alpha rhythm in man.R. Albino & G. Burnand - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (6):539.
  6. (1 other version)Kinesthetic Memory.Maxine Sheets-Johnstone - 2003 - Theoria Et Historia Scientiarum 7 (1):69-92.
    This paper attempts to elucidate the nature of kinesthetic memory, demonstrate itscentrality to everyday human movement, and thereby promote fresh cognitive andphenomenological understandings of movement in everyday life. Prominent topics in this undertaking include kinesthesia, dynamics, and habit. The endeavor has both a critical and constructive dimension.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  7. The changing of perceived speed as a function of stimulus contrast: an attempted replication with a variety of stimuli.M. R. Blakemore & R. J. Snowden - 1996 - In Enrique Villanueva (ed.), Perception. Ridgeview Pub. Co. pp. 34-34.
  8.  80
    S-r compatibility: Correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes.Paul M. Fitts & Richard L. Deininger - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (6):483.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  9.  59
    Category rating scales: Effects of relative spacing and frequency of stimulus values.Allen Parducci & Linda F. Perrett - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 89 (2):427.
  10.  46
    Kinesthetic-visual matching and the self-concept as explanations of mirror-self-recognition.Robert W. Mitchell - 1997 - Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 27 (1):17–39.
    Since its inception as a topic of inquiry, mirror-self-recognition has usually been explained by two models: one, initiated by Guillaume, proposes that mirror-self-recognition depends upon kinesthetic-visual matching, and the other, initiated by Gallup, that self-recognition depends upon a self-concept. These two models are examined historically and conceptually. This examination suggests that the kinesthetic-visual matching model is conceptually coherent and makes reasonable and accurate predictions; and that the self-concept model is conceptually incoherent and makes inaccurate predictions from premises which (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  11.  23
    Kinesthetic figural aftereffects: Satiation or contrast.Joseph J. Moylan - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (1):83.
  12.  56
    External control of the stream of consciousness: Stimulus-based effects on involuntary thought sequences.Christina Merrick, Melika Farnia, Tiffany K. Jantz, Adam Gazzaley & Ezequiel Morsella - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 33:217-225.
  13.  29
    Mechanisms of auditory backward masking in the stimulus suffix effect.Robert G. Crowder - 1978 - Psychological Review 85 (6):502-524.
  14. Induction of relations between continuous stimulus and response dimensions.K. Koh & De Meyer - 1989 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27 (6):495-495.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  31
    Kinesthetic Unity as Motivated Association.Andrea Lanza - 2020 - Gestalt Theory 42 (3):271-286.
    Summary Within Husserl’s theory of perception, the role attributed to kinesthetic sensations determines a phase of the perceptive constitution that marks the boundary between pure receptivity and a first form of self-determination of consciousness. Kinesthetic experiences are, in fact, characterized not just as acts that are performed but rather that can be performed, albeit according to predetermined paths. This primitive form of ‘instinctive’ spontaneity of the Ego (linked to primal impulses) as realization of pre-established potentialities, characterizes what Husserl (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  16.  49
    Kinesthetic and vestibular information modulate alpha activity during spatial navigation: a mobile EEG study.Benedikt V. Ehinger, Petra Fischer, Anna L. Gert, Lilli Kaufhold, Felix Weber, Gordon Pipa & Peter König - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
  17.  35
    Absolute judgments as a function of stimulus range and number of stimulus and response categories.Charles W. Eriksen & Harold W. Hake - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 49 (5):323.
  18.  17
    The effects of nonreinforced and randomly reinforced stimulus preexposure on conditioned suppression in rats.Mary Shore Logan & Paul Schnur - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 8 (4):336-338.
  19.  17
    Information processing of olfactory stimuli by the dog: II. Stimulus control and sampling strategies in simultaneous discrimination learning.R. E. Lubow, Moshe Kahn & Reuven Frommer - 1976 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 8 (4):323-326.
  20.  45
    A study of concept formation as a function of reinforcement and stimulus generalization.Arnold H. Buss - 1950 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 40 (4):494.
  21.  20
    Sensitization of the Beta-response as a function of the wavelength of the stimulus.David A. Grant, John P. Hornseth & Harold W. Hake - 1949 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 39 (2):195.
  22.  33
    Studies in incidental learning: IV. The interaction of orienting tasks and stimulus materials.Leo Postman & Pauline Austin Adams - 1956 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 51 (5):329.
  23.  24
    Reactions toward the apparent source of an auditory stimulus.J. Richard Simon, John L. Craft & A. M. Small - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 89 (1):203.
  24.  28
    The problem of stimulation deafness. III. The functional and histological effects of a high-frequency stimulus.Kendon R. Smith & Ernest Glen Wever - 1949 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 39 (2):238.
  25.  26
    Transposition in the white rat as a function of stimulus comparison.Robert Thompson - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 50 (3):185.
  26.  29
    The relation between reaction time and temporal location of the stimulus on the tremor cycle.J. Tiffin & F. L. Westhafer - 1940 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 27 (3):318.
  27.  26
    Covert preparation of a manual response in a ‘go’/‘no-go’ saccadic task is driven by execution of the eye movement and not by visual stimulus occurrence.Claudio Maioli & Luca Falciati - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  28.  24
    Kinesthetic retention, movement extent, and information processing.George E. Stelmach & Mark Wilson - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 85 (3):425.
  29.  30
    Subjective Vividness of Kinesthetic Motor Imagery Is Associated With the Similarity in Magnitude of Sensorimotor Event-Related Desynchronization Between Motor Execution and Motor Imagery.Hisato Toriyama, Junichi Ushiba & Junichi Ushiyama - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12:368189.
    In the field of psychology, it has been well established that there are two types of motor imagery such as kinesthetic motor imagery (KMI) and visual motor imagery (VMI), and the subjective evaluation for vividness of motor imagery each differs across individuals. This study aimed to examine how the motor imagery ability assessed by the psychological scores is associated with the physiological measure using electroencephalogram (EEG) sensorimotor rhythm during KMI task. First, 20 healthy young individuals evaluated subjectively how vividly (...)
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  30.  40
    Distinct Developmental Changes in Auditory and Somatosensory N1 ERP Enhancements at Rapid Stimulus Intervals.Wright Megan, Timora Justin, Paton Bryan & Budd Timothy - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  31.  21
    Semantic Negative Priming From an Ignored Single-Prime Depends Critically on Prime-Mask Inter-Stimulus Interval and Working Memory Capacity.Montserrat Megías, Juan J. Ortells, Carmen Noguera, Isabel Carmona & Paloma Marí-Beffa - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11:512601.
    The aim of this study is to examine the link between working memory capacity and the ability to exert cognitive control. Here, participants with either high or low working memory capacity (WMC) performed a semantic negative priming (NP) task as a measure of cognitive control. They were required to ignore a single prime word followed by a pattern mask appearing immediately or after a delay. The prime could be semantically related or unrelated to an upcoming target word where a forced-choice (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32. Kinesthetic Empathy, Dance, and Technology.Andrew J. Corsa - 2016 - Polymath: An Interdisciplinary Arts and Sciences Journal 6 (2):1-34.
    I argue that when we use email, text messaging, or social media websites such as Facebook to interact, rather than communicating face-to-face, we do not experience the best kind of empathy, which is most conducive to experiencing benevolence for others. My arguments rely on drawing interdisciplinary connections between sources: early modern accounts of sympathy, dance theory, philosophy of technology, and neuroscience/psychology. I reflect on theories from these disciplines which, taken together, suggest that to empathize optimally, we must see or hear (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  33.  41
    Chronic effects of a high-frequency stimulus on the structure and function of the cochlea.Irving E. Alexander & Frederick J. Githler - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 49 (5):363.
  34.  24
    Expectancy in choice reaction time: Anticipation of stimulus or response?James V. Hinrichs & Patricia L. Krainz - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 85 (3):330.
  35.  66
    Studies in thermal sensitivity: 16. Further evidence on the effects of stimulus temperature.W. L. Jenkins - 1941 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 29 (5):413.
  36.  57
    Appreciating the poverty of the stimulus in second language acquisition.Rex A. Sprouse - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):742-743.
    The most compelling evidence for Epstein et al.'s central thesis that adult second language acquisition is constrained by the innate cognitive structures that constrain native language acquisition would be evidence of poverty of the stimulus. Although there are studies that point to such evidence, Epstein et al.'s primary form of argumentation, targetlike performance by second-language acquiring adults, is much less convincing.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37.  16
    Differentiating aversive conditioning in bistable perception: Avoidance of a percept vs. salience of a stimulus.Gregor Wilbertz & Philipp Sterzer - 2018 - Consciousness and Cognition 61:38-48.
  38.  12
    Kinesthetic memory Further critical reflections and constructive analyses.Maxine Sheets-Iohnstone - 2012 - In Sabine C. Koch, Thomas Fuchs, Michela Summa & Cornelia Müller (eds.), Body Memory, Metaphor and Movement. John Benjamins. pp. 84--43.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  39.  28
    Tactual-kinesthetic feedback from manipulation of visual forms and nondifferential reinforcement in transfer of perceptual learning.Thomas L. Bennett & Henry C. Ellis - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (3p1):495.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  40.  75
    Stimulus, sensation, and meaning.Glenn D. Higginson - 1935 - Journal of Philosophy 32 (24):645-650.
    We can find no place in psychology for the concept of stimulus as a physical agent to which an individual responds in a psychological manner. Moreover, we can find no place for sensation and image when considered as simple mental elements. We would also purge ...
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  60
    Kinesthetic Understanding and Appreciation in Dance.William P. Seeley NoËl Carroll - 2013 - Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 71 (2):177-186.
    The idea that choreographic movements communicate to audiences by kinetic transfer is a commonplace among choreographers, dancers, and dance educators.1 Moreover, most dance lovers can cite their own favorite examples—the bounciness of the Royal Danish Ballet, the stomping of Bharata Natyam performers, the stag leaps in the thundering Greek chorus in Martha Graham’s Night Journey, or the contagious rhythmic transfer that takes over our feet when we watch classic tap dancers like Buster Brown. The perceptual capacity for kinetic transfer was (...)
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  42.  22
    Correlation between visual and kinesthetic spatial aftereffects.A. A. Landauer, G. Singer & R. H. Day - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (6):892.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  43.  27
    Stimulus meaning in stimulus predifferentiation.Sheila M. Pfafflin - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 59 (4):269.
  44.  57
    Stimulus-category competition, inhibition, and affective devaluation: a novel account of the uncanny valley.Anne E. Ferrey, Tyler J. Burleigh & Mark J. Fenske - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:92507.
    Stimuli that resemble humans, but are not perfectly human-like, are disliked compared to distinctly human and nonhuman stimuli. Accounts of this “Uncanny Valley” effect often focus on how changes in human resemblance can evoke different emotional responses. We present an alternate account based on the novel hypothesis that the Uncanny Valley is not directly related to ‘human-likeness’ per se, but instead reflects a more general form of stimulus devaluation that occurs when inhibition is triggered to resolve conflict between competing (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  45.  22
    Stimulus generalization as a function of verbal reinforcement combination.Arnold H. Buss, Morton Weiner & Edith Buss - 1954 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 48 (6):433.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46. Bodily-kinesthetic intelligence and dance education: Critique, revision, and potentials for the democratic ideal.Donald Blumenfeld-Jones - 2009 - Journal of Aesthetic Education 43 (1):pp. 59-76.
  47.  26
    Kinesthetic aftereffects and evoked potentials constitute parallel measures of augmenting-reducing.A. Harvey Baker & Irene W. Kostin - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):744-746.
  48.  14
    Stimulus overlap in a massed-trial situation.Rose Ginsberg - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 67 (6):553.
  49.  28
    Stimulus generalization of an instrumental response as a function of the number of reinforced trials.Garry Margolius - 1955 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 49 (2):105.
  50.  26
    Multidimensional stimulus generalization of a tactile response along the dimensions of angularity and texture.Ronald B. Purtle & Frederick L. Newman - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (3):566.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
1 — 50 / 992